Results for 'Asifa Majid'

281 found
Order:
  1. Can language restructure cognition? The case for space.Asifa Majid, Melissa Bowerman, Sotaro Kita, Daniel B. M. Haun & Stephen C. Levinson - 2004 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (3):108-114.
  2.  49
    Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language.Asifa Majid & Niclas Burenhult - 2014 - Cognition 130 (2):266-270.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  3. Differential Ineffability and the Senses.Stephen C. Levinson & Asifa Majid - 2014 - Mind and Language 29 (4):407-427.
    Ineffability, the degree to which percepts or concepts resist linguistic coding, is a fairly unexplored nook of cognitive science. Although philosophical preoccupations with qualia or nonconceptual content certainly touch upon the area, there has been little systematic thought and hardly any empirical work in recent years on the subject. We argue that ineffability is an important domain for the cognitive sciences. For examining differential ineffability across the senses may be able to tell us important things about how the mind works, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  4.  42
    Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq.Ewelina Wnuk & Asifa Majid - 2014 - Cognition 131 (1):125-138.
  5.  15
    Anger stinks in Seri: Olfactory metaphor in a lesser-described language.Asifa Majid & Carolyn O’Meara - 2020 - Cognitive Linguistics 31 (3):367-391.
    Previous studies claim there are few olfactory metaphors cross-linguistically, especially compared to metaphors originating in the visual and auditory domains. We show olfaction can be a source for metaphor and metonymy in a lesser-described language that has rich lexical resources for talking about odors. In Seri, an isolate language of Mexico spoken by indigenous hunter-gatherers, we find a novel metaphor for emotion never previously described – “anger stinks”. In addition, distinct odor verbs are used metaphorically to distinguish volitional vs. non-volitional (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  6.  50
    The cross-linguistic categorization of everyday events: A study of cutting and breaking.Asifa Majid, James S. Boster & Melissa Bowerman - 2008 - Cognition 109 (2):235-250.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  7.  28
    Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage.Bodo Winter, Marcus Perlman & Asifa Majid - 2018 - Cognition 179 (C):213-220.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  8.  40
    The semantic categories of cutting and breaking events: A crosslinguistic perspective.Asifa Majid, Melissa Bowerman, Miriam van Staden & James S. Boster - 2007 - Cognitive Linguistics 18 (2).
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  9.  70
    Can Nomenclature for the Body be Explained by Embodiment Theories?Asifa Majid & Miriam Staden - 2015 - Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (4):570-594.
    According to widespread opinion, the meaning of body part terms is determined by salient discontinuities in the visual image; such that hands, feet, arms, and legs, are natural parts. If so, one would expect these parts to have distinct names which correspond in meaning across languages. To test this proposal, we compared three unrelated languages—Dutch, Japanese, and Indonesian—and found both naming systems and boundaries of even basic body part terms display variation across languages. Bottom-up cues alone cannot explain natural language (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  10.  7
    Can Nomenclature for the Body be Explained by Embodiment Theories?Asifa Majid & Miriam van Staden - 2015 - Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (4):570-594.
    According to widespread opinion, the meaning of body part terms is determined by salient discontinuities in the visual image; such that hands, feet, arms, and legs, are natural parts. If so, one would expect these parts to have distinct names which correspond in meaning across languages. To test this proposal, we compared three unrelated languages—Dutch, Japanese, and Indonesian—and found both naming systems and boundaries of even basic body part terms display variation across languages. Bottom‐up cues alone cannot explain natural language (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  11.  37
    Current Emotion Research in the Language Sciences.Asifa Majid - 2012 - Emotion Review 4 (4):432-443.
    When researchers think about the interaction between language and emotion, they typically focus on descriptive emotion words. This review demonstrates that emotion can interact with language at many levels of structure, from the sound patterns of a language to its lexicon and grammar, and beyond to how it appears in conversation and discourse. Findings are considered from diverse subfields across the language sciences, including cognitive linguistics, psycholinguistics, linguistic anthropology, and conversation analysis. Taken together, it is clear that emotional expression is (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  12.  49
    The Role of Language in a Science of Emotion.Asifa Majid - 2012 - Emotion Review 4 (4):380-381.
    Emotion scientists often take an ambivalent stance concerning the role of language in a science of emotion. However, it is important for emotion researchers to contemplate some of the consequences of current practices for their theory building. There is a danger of an overreliance on the English language as a transparent window into emotion categories. More consideration has to be given to cross-linguistic comparison in the future so that models of language acquisition and of the language–cognition interface fit better the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  13.  94
    WEIRD languages have misled us, too.Asifa Majid & Stephen C. Levinson - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):103-103.
    The linguistic and cognitive sciences have severely underestimated the degree of linguistic diversity in the world. Part of the reason for this is that we have projected assumptions based on English and familiar languages onto the rest. We focus on some distortions this has introduced, especially in the study of semantics.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  14.  36
    An Exception to Mental Simulation: No Evidence for Embodied Odor Language.Laura J. Speed & Asifa Majid - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (4):1146-1178.
    Do we mentally simulate olfactory information? We investigated mental simulation of odors and sounds in two experiments. Participants retained a word while they smelled an odor or heard a sound, then rated odor/sound intensity and recalled the word. Later odor/sound recognition was also tested, and pleasantness and familiarity judgments were collected. Word recall was slower when the sound and sound-word mismatched. Sound recognition was higher when sounds were paired with a match or near-match word. This indicates sound-words are mentally simulated. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  15.  36
    How similar are semantic categories in closely related languages? A comparison of cutting and breaking in four Germanic languages.Asifa Majid, Marianne Gullberg, Miriam van Staden & Melissa Bowerman - 2007 - Cognitive Linguistics 18 (2).
  16.  42
    Do Language-Specific Categories Shape Conceptual Processing? Mandarin Classifier Distinctions Influence Eye Gaze Behavior, but only During Linguistic Processing.Falk Huettig, Asifa Majid, Jidong Chen & Melissa Bowerman - 2010 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 10 (1-2):39-58.
    In two eye-tracking studies we investigated the influence of Mandarin numeral classifiers – a grammatical category in the language – on online overt attention. Mandarin speakers were presented with simple sentences through headphones while their eye-movements to objects presented on a computer screen were monitored. The crucial question is what participants look at while listening to a pre-specified target noun. If classifier categories influence Mandarin speakers' general conceptual processing, then on hearing the target noun they should look at objects that (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  17.  28
    Iranian Herbalists, But Not Cooks, Are Better at Naming Odors Than Laypeople.Marisa Casillas, Afrooz Rafiee & Asifa Majid - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (6):e12763.
    Odor naming is enhanced in communities where communication about odors is a central part of daily life (e.g., wine experts, flavorists, and some hunter‐gatherer groups). In this study, we investigated how expert knowledge and daily experience affect the ability to name odors in a group of experts that has not previously been investigated in this context—Iranian herbalists; also called attars—as well as cooks and laypeople. We assessed naming accuracy and consistency for 16 herb and spice odors, collected judgments of odor (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  9
    Consistent verbal labels promote odor category learning.Norbert Vanek, Márton Sóskuthy & Asifa Majid - 2021 - Cognition 206 (C):104485.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19.  54
    Language does provide support for basic tastes.Asifa Majid & Stephen C. Levinson - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):86-87.
    Recurrent lexicalization patterns across widely different cultural contexts can provide a window onto common conceptualizations. The cross-linguistic data support the idea that sweet, salt, sour, and bitter are basic tastes. In addition, umami and fatty are likely basic tastes, as well.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  20.  67
    A crosslinguistic perspective on semantic cognition.Asifa Majid & Falk Huettig - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):720-721.
    Coherent covariation appears to be a powerful explanatory factor accounting for a range of phenomena in semantic cognition. But its role in accounting for the crosslinguistic facts is less clear. Variation in naming, within the same semantic domain, raises vexing questions about the necessary parameters needed to account for the basic facts underlying categorization.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  46
    Covariation and quantifier polarity: What determines causal attribution in vignettes?Asifa Majid, Anthony J. Sanford & Martin J. Pickering - 2006 - Cognition 99 (1):35-51.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  13
    Nicholas Evans.Asifa Majid, Christian J. Rapold, Peter Hurst, Ulrike Zeshan, Toshiki Osada, N. J. Enfield, Nicole Kruspe & Niclas Burenhult - 2011 - In Nicholas Evans (ed.), Reciprocals and Semantic Typology. John Benjamins Pub. Company. pp. 341.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  10
    Patterns of semantic variation differ across body parts: evidence from the Japonic languages.Asifa Majid, Roeland van Hout & John L. A. Huisman - 2021 - Cognitive Linguistics 32 (3):455-486.
    The human body is central to myriad metaphors, so studying the conceptualisation of the body itself is critical if we are to understand its broader use. One essential but understudied issue is whether languages differ in which body parts they single out for naming. This paper takes a multi-method approach to investigate body part nomenclature within a single language family. Using both a naming task and colouring-in task to collect data from six Japonic languages, we found that lexical similarity for (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  10
    The semantics of reciprocal constructions across languages.Asifa Majid, Nicholas Evans, Alice Gaby & Stephen C. Levinson - 2011 - In Nicholas Evans (ed.), Reciprocals and Semantic Typology. John Benjamins Pub. Company. pp. 29.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  8
    Evidence for a Shared Instrument Prototype from English, Dutch, and German.Lilia Rissman, Saskia Putten & Asifa Majid - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (5):e13140.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 5, May 2022.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26. Spatial terms reflect near-optimal spatial categories.Naveen Khetarpal, Asifa Majid & Terry Regier - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 2396--2401.
  27.  16
    Universal meaning extensions of perception verbs are grounded in interaction.Lila San Roque, Kobin H. Kendrick, Elisabeth Norcliffe & Asifa Majid - 2018 - Cognitive Linguistics 29 (3):371-406.
    Apart from references to perception, words such as see and listen have shared, non-literal meanings across diverse languages. Such cross-linguistic meanings have not been systematically investigated as they appear in their natural home — informal spoken interaction. We present a qualitative examination of the semantic associations of perception verbs based on recorded everyday conversation in thirteen diverse languages. Across these diverse communities, spontaneous interaction provides evidence for two commonly-discussed extensions of perception verbs — perception~cognition, hearing~linguistic communication — as well as (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  28.  19
    Space-pitch associations differ in their susceptibility to language.Sarah Dolscheid, Simge Çelik, Hasan Erkan, Aylin Küntay & Asifa Majid - 2020 - Cognition 196 (C):104073.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  29.  12
    Embodied Space‐pitch Associations are Shaped by Language.Judith Holler, Linda Drijvers, Afrooz Rafiee & Asifa Majid - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13083.
    Height-pitch associations are claimed to be universal and independent of language, but this claim remains controversial. The present study sheds new light on this debate with a multimodal analysis of individual sound and melody descriptions obtained in an interactive communication paradigm with speakers of Dutch and Farsi. The findings reveal that, in contrast to Dutch speakers, Farsi speakers do not use a height-pitch metaphor consistently in speech. Both Dutch and Farsi speakers’ co-speech gestures did reveal a mapping of higher pitches (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  30.  4
    Embodied Space‐pitch Associations are Shaped by Language.Judith Holler, Linda Drijvers, Afrooz Rafiee & Asifa Majid - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13083.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 2, February 2022.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  31.  10
    Expertise Shapes Multimodal Imagery for Wine.Ilja Croijmans, Laura J. Speed, Artin Arshamian & Asifa Majid - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (5):e12842.
    Although taste and smell seem hard to imagine, some people nevertheless report vivid imagery in these sensory modalities. We investigate whether experts are better able to imagine smells and tastes because they have learned the ability, or whether they are better imaginers in the first place, and so become experts. To test this, we first compared a group of wine experts to yoked novices using a battery of questionnaires. We show for the first time that experts report greater vividness of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  32.  9
    The Sound of Smell: Associating Odor Valence With Disgust Sounds.Laura J. Speed, Hannah Atkinson, Ewelina Wnuk & Asifa Majid - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (5):e12980.
    Olfaction has recently been highlighted as a sense poorly connected with language. Odor is difficult to verbalize, and it has few qualities that afford mimicry by vision or sound. At the same time, emotion is thought to be the most salient dimension of an odor, and it could therefore be an olfactory dimension more easily communicated. We investigated whether sounds imitative of an innate disgust response can be associated with unpleasant odors. In two experiments, participants were asked to make a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  33.  72
    Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies.Lila San Roque, Kobin H. Kendrick, Elisabeth Norcliffe, Penelope Brown, Rebecca Defina, Mark Dingemanse, Tyko Dirksmeyer, N. J. Enfield, Simeon Floyd, Jeremy Hammond, Giovanni Rossi, Sylvia Tufvesson, Saskia van Putten & Asifa Majid - 2015 - Cognitive Linguistics 26 (1):31-60.
    To what extent does perceptual language reflect universals of experience and cognition, and to what extent is it shaped by particular cultural preoccupations? This paper investigates the universality~relativity of perceptual language by examining the use of basic perception terms in spontaneous conversation across 13 diverse languages and cultures. We analyze the frequency of perception words to test two universalist hypotheses: that sight is always a dominant sense, and that the relative ranking of the senses will be the same across different (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  34.  24
    Manners of human gait: a crosslinguistic event-naming study.Dan I. Slobin, Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Anetta Kopecka & Asifa Majid - 2014 - Cognitive Linguistics 25 (4).
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 25 Heft: 4 Seiten: 701-741.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  35.  16
    Putting and taking events.Bhuvana Narasimhan, Anetta Kopecka, Melissa Bowerman, Marianne Gullberg & Asifa Majid - 2012 - In Anetta Kopecka & Bhuvana Narasimhan (eds.), Events of "Putting" and "Taking": A Crosslinguistic Perspective. John Benjamins. pp. 1.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  36.  43
    Antecedent frequency effects during the processing of pronouns.Roger P. G. van Gompel & Asifa Majid - 2004 - Cognition 90 (3):255-264.
  37.  32
    Shades of emotion: What the addition of sunglasses or masks to faces reveals about the development of facial expression processing.Debi Roberson, Mariko Kikutani, Paula Döge, Lydia Whitaker & Asifa Majid - 2012 - Cognition 125 (2):195-206.
  38.  9
    Evidence for a Shared Instrument Prototype from English, Dutch, and German.Lilia Rissman, Saskia van Putten & Asifa Majid - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (5):e13140.
    At conceptual and linguistic levels of cognition, events are said to be represented in terms of abstract categories, for example, the sentence Jackie cut the bagel with a knife encodes the categories Agent (i.e., Jackie) and Patient (i.e., the bagel). In this paper, we ask whether entities such as the knife are also represented in terms of such a category (often labeled “Instrument”) and, if so, whether this category has a prototype structure. We hypothesized the Proto-instrument is a tool: a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  6
    Odor‐Color Associations Are Not Mediated by Concurrent Verbalization.Laura J. Speed, Josje de Valk, Ilja Croijmans, John L. A. Huisman & Asifa Majid - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (4):e13266.
    Odor and color are strongly associated. Numerous studies demonstrate consistent odor‐color associations, as well as effects of color on odor perception and language. Yet, we know little about how these associations arise. Here, we test whether language is a possible mediator of odor‐color associations, specifically whether odor‐color associations are mediated by implicit odor naming. In two experiments, we used an interference paradigm to prevent the verbalization of odors during an odor‐color matching task. If participants generate color associations subsequent to labeling (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  7
    Color technology is not necessary for rich and efficient color language.Ewelina Wnuk, Annemarie Verkerk, Stephen C. Levinson & Asifa Majid - 2022 - Cognition 229 (C):105223.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  48
    Bridging the Gap: From Cognitive Anthropology to Cognitive Science.Andrea Bender, Sieghard Beller, Giovanni Bennardo, James S. Boster, Asifa Majid & Douglas L. Medin - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.
  42.  12
    Lack of Visual Experience Affects Multimodal Language Production: Evidence From Congenitally Blind and Sighted People.Ezgi Mamus, Laura J. Speed, Lilia Rissman, Asifa Majid & Aslı Özyürek - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (1):e13228.
    The human experience is shaped by information from different perceptual channels, but it is still debated whether and how differential experience influences language use. To address this, we compared congenitally blind, blindfolded, and sighted people's descriptions of the same motion events experienced auditorily by all participants (i.e., via sound alone) and conveyed in speech and gesture. Comparison of blind and sighted participants to blindfolded participants helped us disentangle the effects of a lifetime experience of being blind versus the task-specific effects (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  39
    The Benacerraf Problem as a Challenge for Ontic Structural Realism.Majid Davoody Beni - 2020 - Philosophia Mathematica 28 (1):35-59.
    Benacerraf has presented two problems for the philosophy of mathematics. These are the problem of identification and the problem of representation. This paper aims to reconstruct the latter problem and to unpack its undermining bearing on the version of Ontic Structural Realism that frames scientific representations in terms of abstract structures. I argue that the dichotomy between mathematical structures and physical ones cannot be used to address the Benacerraf problem but strengthens it. I conclude by arguing that versions of OSR (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44. Ab'ād al-tajribah al-falsafīyah.Majid Fakhry - 1980 - Bayrūt,:
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  9
    Fodor and the Impossibility of Learning.Majid Amini - 2011-09-16 - In Michael Bruce & Steven Barbone (eds.), Just the Arguments. Wiley‐Blackwell. pp. 359–361.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  5
    Fodor's Argument for Linguistic Nativism.Majid Amini - 2011-09-16 - In Michael Bruce & Steven Barbone (eds.), Just the Arguments. Wiley‐Blackwell. pp. 355–358.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Arisṭūṭālīs: al-muʻallim al-awwal.Majid Fakhry - 1958 - Bayrūt: al-Maṭbaʻat al-Kāthūlīkīyah.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  4
    Ibn Rushd.Majid Fakhry - 1960
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  89
    Multicultural Nationalism: Islamophobia, Anglophobia, and Devolution.Asifa M. Hussain & William L. Miller - 2006 - Oxford University Press.
    This is a pioneering study of how multiculturalism interacts with multinationalism. Focusing specifically on post-devolution Scotland, and based on statistical analysis of over 1500 interviews, Hussain and Miller critically examine the challenges of Scotland's largest visible and invisible minorities: ethnic Pakistanis and English immigrants.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  50.  52
    Substantial motion, 400 years of wishful thinking!Majid Borumand - manuscript
    The concept of Substantial motion (حركت جوهرى) is fundamentally flawed and severely muddled. Aristotle and Mulla Sadra’s conception of motion, substance (جوهر) and substantial form صورت نوعيه)) were all based on a severe misunderstanding of nature as later was established by the scientists and philosophers that came after them. Here, by recalling the established facts of modern science, particularly the universally accepted scientific fact that, properties of objects are reducible to the motion of their electrons and there’s no such thing (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 281