Results for 'David Gooding'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. SeCoDa: Sense Complexity Dataset.David Strohmaier, Sian Gooding, Shiva Taslimipoor & Ekaterina Kochmar - 2020 - Proceedings of the 12Th Language Resources and Evaluation Conference.
    The Sense Complexity Dataset (SeCoDa) provides a corpus that is annotated jointly for complexity and word senses. It thus provides a valuable resource for both word sense disambiguation and the task of complex word identification. The intention is that this dataset will be used to identify complexity at the level of word senses rather than word tokens. For word sense annotation SeCoDa uses a hierarchical scheme that is based on information available in the Cambridge Advanced Learner’s Dictionary. This way we (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  17
    Moral Judgment and its Impact on Business-to-Business Sales Performance and Customer Relationships.David J. Good & Charles H. Schwepker - 2011 - Journal of Business Ethics 98 (4):609 - 625.
    For many years, researchers and practitioners have sought out meaningful indicators of sales performance. Yet, as the concept of performance has broadened, the understanding of what makes up a successful seller, has become far more complicated. The complexity of buyer-seller relationships has changed therefore as the definition of sales performance has expanded, cultivating a growing interest in ethical/unethical actions since they could potentially have impacts on sales performance. Given this environment, the purpose of this study is to explore the impact (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  3.  8
    David Good, A World Without Words.David Goode - 1997 - Human Studies 20 (3):357-357.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  4.  29
    The Uses of Experiment: Studies in the Natural Sciences.David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer - 1989 - Cambridge University Press. Edited by David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer.
    Contributors; Preface; Introduction; Part I. Instruments in Experiments: 1. Scientific instruments: models of brass and aids to discovery; 2. Glass works: Newton’s prisms and the uses of experiment; 3. A viol of water or a wedge of glass; Part II. Experiment and Argument: 4. Galileo’s experimental discourse; 5. Fresnel, Poisson and the white spot: the role of successful predictions in the acceptance of scientific theories; 6. The rhetoric of experiment; Part III. Representing and Realising: 7. ’Magnetic curves’ and the magnetic (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  5. The Uses of Experiment.David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer - 1992 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 43 (1):99-109.
  6. What is Experimental about Thought Experiments?David C. Gooding - 1992 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1992:280 - 290.
    I argue that thought experiments are a form of experimental reasoning similar to real experiments. They require the same ability to participate by following a narrative as real experiments do. Participation depends in turn on using what we already know to visualize, manipulate and understand what is unfamiliar or problematic. I defend the claim that visualization requires embodiment by an example which shows how tacit understanding of the properties of represented objects and relations enables us to work out how such (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  7.  58
    How do Scientists Reach Agreement about Novel Observations?David Gooding - 1986 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 17 (2):205.
    I outline a pragmatic view of scientists' use of observation which draws attention to non-discursive, instrumental and social contexts of observation, in order to explain scientists' agreement about the appearance and significance of new phenomena. I argue that: observation is embedded in a network of activities, techniques, and interests; that experimentalists make construals of new phenomena which enable them communicate exploratory techniques and their outcomes, and that empirical enquiry consists of communicative, exploratory and predictive strategies whose interdependence ensures that, notwithstanding (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  8.  12
    Empiricism in Practice: Teleology, Economy, and Observation in Faraday's Physics.David Gooding - 1982 - Isis 73:46-67.
  9.  16
    Empiricism in Practice: Teleology, Economy, and Observation in Faraday's Physics.David Gooding - 1982 - Isis 73 (1):46-67.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  10.  77
    Visualizing Scientific Inference.David C. Gooding - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (1):15-35.
    The sciences use a wide range of visual devices, practices, and imaging technologies. This diversity points to an important repertoire of visual methods that scientists use to adapt representations to meet the varied demands that their work places on cognitive processes. This paper identifies key features of the use of visualization in a range of scientific domains and considers the implications of this repertoire for understanding scientists as cognitive agents.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  11.  18
    Cognition, Construction and Culture: Visual Theories in the Sciences.David Gooding - 2004 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 4 (3-4):551-593.
    This paper presents a study of the generation, manipulation and use of visual representations in different episodes of scientific discovery. The study identifies a common set of transformations of visual representations underlying the distinctive methods and imagery of different scientific fields. The existence of common features behind the diversity of visual representations suggests a common dynamical structure for visual thinking, showing how visual representations facilitate cognitive processes such as pattern-matching and visual inference through the use of tools, technologies and other (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  12. The Procedural Turn; or, Why Do Thought Experiments Work?David Gooding - 1992 - In R. Giere & H. Feigl (eds.), Cognitive Models of Science. University of Minnesota Press. pp. 45-76.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  13.  7
    Mapping Experiment as a Learning Process: How the First Electromagnetic Motor Was Invented.David Gooding - 1990 - Science, Technology and Human Values 15 (2):165-201.
    Narrative accounts misrepresent discovery by reconstructing worlds ordered by success rather than the world as explored. Such worlds rarely contain the personal knowledge that informed actual exploration and experiment. This article describes an attempt to recover situated learning in a material environment, tracing the discovery of the first electromagnetic motor by Michael Faraday in September 1821 to show how he modeled new experience and invented procedures to communicate that novelty. The author introduces a notation to map experiment as an active (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  14.  71
    From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday's visual reasoning.David C. Gooding - 2006 - Perspectives on Science 14 (1):40-65.
    : Faraday is often described as an experimentalist, but his work is a dialectical interplay of concrete objects, visual images, abstract, theoretically-informed visual models and metaphysical precepts. From phenomena described in terms of patterns formed by lines of force he created a general explanation of space-filling systems of force which obey both empirical laws and principles of conservation and economy. I argue that Faraday's articulation of situated experience via visual models into a theory capable of verbal expression owed much to (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  15.  27
    Creative Rationality: towards an Abductive Model of Scientific Change.David Gooding - 1996 - Philosophica 58 (2).
  16.  8
    Semi-Automated Care: Video-Algorithmic Patient Monitoring and Surveillance in Care Settings.Piers M. Gooding & David M. Clifford - 2021 - Journal of Bioethical Inquiry 18 (4):541-546.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17. How Scientists Reach Agreement about New Observations.David Gooding - 1986 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986:236-244.
    I outline a pragmatic view of scientists' use of observation which draws attention to non-discursive, instrumental and social contexts of observation, in order to explain scientists' agreement about the appearance and significance of new phenomena. I argue that: observation is embedded in a network of activities, techniques, and interests; that experimentalists make construals of new phenomena which enable them communicate exploratory techniques and their outcomes, and that empirical enquiry consists of communicative, exploratory and predictive strategies whose interdependence ensures that, notwithstanding (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  18.  20
    Metaphysics versus measurement: The conversion and conservation of force in Faraday's physics.David Gooding - 1980 - Annals of Science 37 (1):1-29.
    SummaryFaraday's concept of force is described by six assumptions. These specify a concept that is quite distinct from ‘mechanical’ conceptions of his contemporaries and interpreters. Analysis of the role of these assumptions clarifies Faraday's weighting of experimental evidence and shows how closely-linked Faraday's chemistry and physics were to his theology. It is argued that Faraday was unable to secularize his concept of force by breaking the ties between his physics and his theology of nature. Examination of his basic assumptions also (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  19.  70
    Visual cognition: Where cognition and culture meet.David C. Gooding - 2006 - Philosophy of Science 73 (5):688-698.
    Case studies of diverse scientific fields show how scientists use a range of resources to generate new interpretative models and to establish their plausibility as explanations of a domain. They accomplish this by manipulating imagistic representations in particular ways. I show that scientists in different domains use the same basic transformations. Common features of these transformations indicate that general cognitive strategies of interpretation, simplification, elaboration, and argumentation are at work. Social and historical studies of science emphasize the diversity of local (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  20.  48
    Kids, culture and innocents.David A. Goode - 1986 - Human Studies 9 (1):83 - 106.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  21. Seeing the forest for the trees: Visualization, cognition, and scientific inference.David C. Gooding - 2005 - In M. Gorman, R. Tweney, D. Gooding & A. Kincannon (eds.), Scientific and Technological Thinking. Erlbaum. pp. 2005--173.
  22.  32
    Conceptual and Experimental Bases of Faraday's Denial of Electrostatic Action at a Distance.David Gooding - 1978 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 9 (2):117.
  23.  16
    Field observations on feeding behavior in an Aruba Island rattlesnake : Strike-induced chemosensory searching and trail following.Matthew Goode, Charles W. Radcliffe, Karen Estep, Andrew Odum & David Chiszar - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (4):312-314.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  24.  33
    Faraday, Thomson, and the Concept of the Magnetic Field.David Gooding - 1980 - British Journal for the History of Science 13 (2):91-120.
    In June 1849 William Thomson wrote to Michael Faraday suggesting that the concept of a uniform magnetic field could be used to predict the motions of small magnetic and diamagnetic bodies. In his letter Thomson showed how Faraday's lines of magnetic force could represent the effect of the ‘conducting power’ for magnetic force of matter in the region of magnets. This was Thomson's extension to magnetism of an analogy between the mathematical descriptions of the distribution of static electricity and of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  25.  17
    Special section: Lorenzo Simpson's The Unfinished Project: `Y'all don't hear me now': On Lorenzo Simpson's The Unfinished Project.Robert Gooding-Williams, Robert Bernasconi, Kenneth Baynes, David M. Rasmussen & Lorenzo C. Simpson - 2007 - Philosophy and Social Criticism 33 (3):289-299.
  26.  5
    How Scientists reach Agreement about new Observations.David Gooding - 1986 - PSA Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986 (1):236-244.
    Epistemology has been socialised. Cognitive and social interactions between observers are now as important as their interventions into the course of nature. This contrasts with traditional views of how we get natural facts into our discourse about nature. These assumed the efficacy of individual observers’ causal interactions with the natural world, making their interactions with other observers irrelevant. Kant’s conclusion that empirical access depends also upon our concepts did not challenge the individualistic character of the epistemology of science. Observers’ agreement (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  11
    Experiment.David C. Gooding - 2017 - In W. H. Newton‐Smith (ed.), A Companion to the Philosophy of Science. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. pp. 117–126.
    There have been many images of experiment. The contemplative narratives of Aristotle served to illustrate hypotheses and arguments. There was no expectation that they be performed. Even in Galileo's dialogues, the distinction between real experiments and imaginary ones is not sharp (see galileo). During the seventeenth century, performance and public description became essential to the probative power of experiment. These made its methods and procedures transparent, allowing any reader of the narrative to be a virtual witness of an active demonstration (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  3
    Cockroft and the AtomGuy Hartcup T. E. Allibone.David Gooding - 1985 - Isis 76 (4):610-611.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  29
    Democracy’s History of Inegalitarianism: Symposium on Michael Hanchard, The Spectre of Race: How Discrimination Haunts Western Democracy, Princeton University Press, 2018.Robert Gooding-Williams, David Theo Goldberg, Juliet Hooker & Michael G. Hanchard - 2020 - Political Theory 48 (3):357-377.
  30.  13
    Energy: Historical Development of the ConceptR. Bruce Lindsay.David C. Gooding - 1977 - Isis 68 (3):464-465.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Faraday and the Powers of Matter the Role of Principles, Hypotheses, and the Interpretation of Experiment in the Development of Faraday's Field Theory, as Presented in His Experimental Researches in Electricity, 1830-1855.David Gooding - 1975
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  3
    Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific TheoriesNancy J. Nersessian.David Gooding - 1986 - Isis 77 (3):521-523.
  33. Review of U nderstanding Philosophy of Science.David Gooding - 2003 - Philosophical Books 44 (3):291-291.
  34.  3
    Re-presenting Faraday.David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the History of Science 27 (3):361-364.
  35.  19
    Some Memories of Harold Garfinkel.David A. Goode - 2012 - Human Studies 35 (2):169-173.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  6
    Scientific Practice: Theories and Stories of Doing Physics. Jed Z. Buchwald.David Gooding - 1997 - Isis 88 (1):121-122.
  37.  40
    Theory and observation: The experimental nexus.David Gooding - 1990 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 4 (2):131 – 148.
    Abstract Philosophical discussions of experiment usually focus exclusively on testing predictions. In this paper I compare G. Morpurgo's experimental test of the Gell?Mann/ Zweig quark hypothesis with two neglected uses of experiment: constructing representations of new phenomena and inventing the instruments that produce such phenomena. These roles are illustrated by J. B. Biot's 1821 observations of electromagnetism and by Michael Faraday's invention of the first electromagnetic motor, also in 1821. The comparison identifies similarities between observation and experiment, showing how both (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  38.  7
    The Concept of Matter in Modern PhilosophyErnan McMullin.David C. Gooding - 1980 - Isis 71 (3):486-486.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  21
    The Rediscovery of the Mind. John R. Searle.David C. Gooding - 1994 - Isis 85 (2):362-363.
  40.  35
    What can particle physicists count on?David Gooding, William J. McKinney, Harry M. Marks, Jeff Hughes & Alan Chalmers - 1999 - Metascience 8 (3):356-392.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  38
    Pragmatics and presence.David Good - 1996 - AI and Society 10 (3-4):309-314.
    This paper considers the potentially important role played by non-verbal communication in constraining pragmatic processing. Attention is paid to claims about the role of emotion in memory encoding and recall, its role in the formulation of plans and goals, and the creation of a shared emotional sense through various interpersonal processes. It is argued that ignoring these factors can lead to pragmatic theories which overestimate the processing demands facing the conversationalist, and that this overestimation will be problematic for any systems (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42. Experiment Right or Wrong.Allan Franklin & David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (1):341-352.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  43.  10
    Esperimento e realismo scientifico: saggio su David Gooding.Marco Buzzoni & David Gooding - 2001 - Napoli: Loffredo. Edited by David Gooding.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. Imaginary science. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (4):1029-1045.
  45.  27
    How to Be a Good Empiricist. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1989 - British Journal for the History of Science 22 (4):419-427.
  46.  9
    Moral Judgment and its Impact on Business-to-Business Sales Performance and Customer Relationships.Charles H. Schwepker & David J. Good - 2011 - Journal of Business Ethics 98 (4):609-625.
    For many years, researchers and practitioners have sought out meaningful indicators of sales performance. Yet, as the concept of performance has broadened, the understanding of what makes up a successful seller, has become far more complicated. The complexity of buyer–seller relationships has changed therefore as the definition of sales performance has expanded, cultivating a growing interest in ethical/unethical actions since they could potentially have impacts on sales performance. Given this environment, the purpose of this study is to explore the impact (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  47.  16
    Moral Judgment and its Impact on Business-to-Business Sales Performance and Customer Relationships.Charles H. Schwepker & David J. Good - 2011 - Journal of Business Ethics 98 (4):609-625.
    For many years, researchers and practitioners have sought out meaningful indicators of sales performance. Yet, as the concept of performance has broadened, the understanding of what makes up a successful seller, has become far more complicated. The complexity of buyer–seller relationships has changed therefore as the definition of sales performance has expanded, cultivating a growing interest in ethical/unethical actions since they could potentially have impacts on sales performance. Given this environment, the purpose of this study is to explore the impact (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  48.  10
    Review: Imaginary Science. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (4):1029 - 1045.
  49. Epistemology of disagreement : the good news.David Christensen - 2018 - In Jeremy Fantl, Matthew McGrath & Ernest Sosa (eds.), Contemporary epistemology: an anthology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
    How should one react when one has a belief, but knows that other people—who have roughly the same evidence as one has, and seem roughly as likely to react to it correctly—disagree? This paper argues that the disagreement of other competent inquirers often requires one to be much less confident in one’s opinions than one would otherwise be.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  50.  7
    Remedial Training of the Less-Impaired Arm in Chronic Stroke Survivors With Moderate to Severe Upper-Extremity Paresis Improves Functional Independence: A Pilot Study.Candice Maenza, David A. Wagstaff, Rini Varghese, Carolee Winstein, David C. Good & Robert L. Sainburg - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    The ipsilesional arm of stroke patients often has functionally limiting deficits in motor control and dexterity that depend on the side of the brain that is lesioned and that increase with the severity of paretic arm impairment. However, remediation of the ipsilesional arm has yet to be integrated into the usual standard of care for upper limb rehabilitation in stroke, largely due to a lack of translational research examining the effects of ipsilesional-arm intervention. We now ask whether ipsilesional-arm training, tailored (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 1000