Abraham, Tara H. (2012). Transcending disciplines: Scientific styles in studies of the brain in mid-twentieth century America. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C 43 (2):552-568.
Abraham, Tara H. (2003). From theory to data: Representing neurons in the 1940s. Biology and Philosophy 18 (3):415-426.
Adams, David B. (1979). Motivational systems, motivational mechanisms, and aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):230-241.
Allen, Colin, Macaque mirror neurons.
Amos, A. J. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2000). The organization of organization: Neuronal scaffold or cognitive straitjacket? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):533-534.
Arbib, Michael A. (1989). Modularity, schemas and neurons: A critique of Fodor. In Peter Slezak (ed.), Computers, Brains and Minds. Kluwer. 193--219.
Aronson, Jerrold L. (1976). Some dubious neurological assumptions of radical behaviourism. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 6 (1):49–60.
Atmanspacher, Harald, Interpreting neurodynamics: Concepts and facts.
Badgaiyan, Rajendra (2012). Nonconscious processing and a novel target for schizophrenia research. Open Journal of Psychiatry 2:335-339.
Baker, LR, A neurocomputational perspective - the nature of mind and the structure of science - Churchland,pm.
Baker, Lynne Rudder, Neuroscience and the Human Mind.
Barclay, Michael W. (1995). The theory of neuronal group selection and its implications for psychology: A critique of the biological self. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 15 (1):41-57.
Bard, Jennifer S. (2007). Learning from Law's Past: A Call for Caution in Incorporating New Innovations in Neuroscience. American Journal of Bioethics 7 (9):73-75.
Bartol, Jordan & Linquist, Stefan (forthcoming). How do Somatic Markers Feature in Decision Making? Emotion Review.
Baumgartner, Michael & Gebharter, Alexander (forthcoming). Constitutive Relevance, Mutual Manipulability, and Fat-Handedness. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science:axv003.
Bechtel, William P., Mental mechanisms: What are the operations?
Bechtel, William P. (2005). The challenge of characterizing operations in the mechanisms underlying behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 84:313-325.
Bechtel, William P. (2002). Aligning Multiple Research Techniques in Cognitive Neuroscience: Why Is It Important? Philosophy of Science 69 (S3):S48-S58.
Bechtel, William P. (2001). Cognitive neuroscienec: Relating neural mechanisms and cognition. In Peter K. Machamer, Peter McLaughlin & Rick Grush (eds.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Press.
Bechtel, William P. (1983). A bridge between cognitive science and neuroscience: The functional architecture of mind. Philosophical Studies 44 (November):319-30.
Bechtel, William P. & Mundale, Jennifer (1996). Integrating neuroscience, psychology, and evolutionary biology through a teleological conception of function. Minds and Machines 6 (4):481-505.
Bechtel, William & Sheredos, Benjamin, HIT on the Psychometric Approach.
Bickle, John (2001). Understanding neural complexity: A role for reduction. Minds and Machines 11 (4):467-481.
Blanc, Francois (2010). Trance and shamanic cure on the south american continent: Psychopharmacological and neurobiological interpretations. Anthropology of Consciousness 21 (1):83-105.
Blumenthal, Terry & Schirillo, James (1999). Biological neuroscience is only as radical as the evolution of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):831-831.
Boyle, Noel (2008). Neurobiology and phenomenology: Towards a three-tiered intertheoretic model of explanation. Journal of Consciousness Studies 15 (3):34-58.
Bradnan, William Andrew (1982). On behavioristic versus neurophysiologic accounts of psychotic behavior. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 7 (3):289-303.
Brothers, Leslie (1999). The logic of interests in neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):831-832.
Brown, Bruce L. ; Hedges, Dawson W. & Gantt, Edwin E. (2008). Brain processes and holistic isomorphism: Moving toward a humanistic neuroscience. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 28 (2):356-374.
Bunge, Silvia A. & Souza, Michael J. (2008). Neural representations used to specify action. In Silvia A. Bunge & Jonathan D. Wallis (eds.), Neuroscience of Rule-Guided Behavior. Oxford University Press.
Burock, Marc, Information and the function of neurons.
Burock, Marc, Evidence for Information Processing in the Brain.
Carleton, Lawrence R. (1985). Levels in Description and Explanation. Philosophy Research Archives 11:89-109.
Carlos M., Muñoz-Suárez & René J., Campis (2008). DID I DO IT? -YEAH, YOU DID! Reduction and Elimination in Philosophy and the Sciences:34- 37.
Changeux, Jean Pierre ; Courrège, Philippe & Danchin, Antoine (1973). A theory of the epigenesis of neuronal networks by selective stabilization of synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Usa 70 (10):2974-8.
Chapeau-Blondeau, François (1995). Information processing in neural networks by means of controlled dynamic regimes. Acta Biotheoretica 43 (1-2):155-167.
Chirimuuta, M. (2013). Extending, changing, and explaining the brain. Biology and Philosophy 28 (4):613-638.
Cliff, D. (1990). Computational Neuroethology: A Provisional Manifesto. In Jean-Arcady Meyer & Stewart W. Wilson (eds.), From Animals to Animats: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior (Complex Adaptive Systems). Cambridge University Press.
Colombetti, G. (2013). Some Ideas for the Integration of Neurophenomenology and Affective Neuroscience. Constructivist Foundations 8 (3):288-297.
Coltheart, Max & Davies, Martin (2003). Inference and explanation in cognitive neuropsychology. Cortex 39 (1):188-191.
Conte, Elio (2012). May We Verify Non-Existing Dispersion Free Ensembles By Application of Quantum Mechanics in Experiments at Perceptive and Cognitive Level? Neuroquantology 10 (1):14-19.
Conte, Elio (2011). On the logical origins of quantum mechanics demonstrated by using Clifford algebra. Electronic Journal of Theoretical Physics 8 (25):109-126.
Couch, Mark B. (2011). Mechanisms and Constitutive Relevance. Synthese 183 (3):375-388.
Coulter, Jeff (1995). The informed neuron: Issues in the use of information theory in the behavioral sciences. Minds and Machines 5 (4):583-96.
Craver, Carl, Why the Hodgkin and huxely model does not explain the action potential.
Craver, Carl F. (2008). Physical law and mechanistic explanation in the Hodgkin and Huxley model of the action potential. Philosophy of Science 75 (5):1022-1033.
Craver, Carl F. (2007). Explaining the Brain: Mechanisms and the Mosaic Unity of Neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press ;.
Craver, Carl F. (2005). Beyond reduction: mechanisms, multifield integration and the unity of neuroscience. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C 36 (2):373-395.
Craver, Carl F. (2003). The making of a memory mechanism. Journal of the History of Biology 36 (1):153-95.
Craver, Carl F. (2002). Interlevel experiments and multilevel mechanisms in the neuroscience of memory. Philosophy of Science Supplemental Volume 69 (3):S83-S97.
Craver, Carl F. (2001). Role functions, mechanisms, and hierarchy. Philosophy of Science 68 (1):53-74.
Craver, Carl F. & Bechtel, William (2007). Top-down causation without top-down causes. Biology and Philosophy 22 (4):547-563.
Craver, Carl F. & Darden, Lindley (2001). Discovering mechanisms in neurobiology: The case of spatial memory. In P.K. Machamer, Rick Grush & Peter McLaughlin (eds.), Theory and Method in Neuroscience. Pittsburgh: University of Pitt Press. 112--137.
Cruse, H. (2001). The explanatory power and limits of simulation models in the neurosciences. In Peter K. Machamer, Peter McLaughlin & Rick Grush (eds.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Press. 138--154.
Davies, Martin, Inference and explanation in cognitive neuropsychology.
de Jong, Huib Looren & Schouten, Maurice K. D. (eds.) (2007). The Matter of the Mind: Philosophical Essays on Psychology, Neuroscience and Reduction. Oxford: Blackwell.
Del Pinal, Guillermo & Nathan, Marco J. (2013). There and Up Again: On the Uses and Misuses of Neuroimaging in Psychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 30 (4):233-252.
Delcomyn, Fred (2001). Biorobotic models can contribute to neurobiology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (6):1056-1057.
Demont, Florian (2012). Chomsky's Methodological Naturalism and the Mereological Fallacy. In Piotr Stalmaszcyzk (ed.), Philosophical and Formal Approaches to Linguistic Analysis. Ontos Verlag. 113.
den Bosch, & P., M. (2005). Structures in neuropharmacology. Poznan Studies in the Philosophy of the Sciences and the Humanities 84 (1):343-359.
Dubreuil, Benoît (2010). Reviews: Explaining the Brain: Mechanisms and the Mosaic Unity of Neurosciences, by Carl F. Craver. European Journal of Philosophy 18 (3):471-474.
Eckardvont, Barbara & Poland, Jeffrey S. (2004). Mechanism and Explanation in Cognitive Neuroscience. Philosophy of Science 71 (5):972-984.
Edelman, Shimon ; Fekete, Tomer & Zach, Neta (eds.) (2012). Being in Time: Dynamical Models of Phenomenal Experience. John Benjamins Pub. Co..
Egan, Frances (forthcoming). Function-Theoretic Explanation and the Search for Neural Mechanisms. In David M. Kaplan (ed.), Integrating Mind and Brain Science: Mechanistic Perspectives and Beyond. Oxford University Press.
Egan, Frances (forthcoming). Function-Theoretic Explanation and Neural Mechanisms. In David M. Kaplan (ed.), Integrating Mind and Brain Science: Mechanistic Perspectives and Beyond.
Egan, Frances & Matthews, Robert J. (2006). Doing cognitive neuroscience: A third way. Synthese 153 (3):377-391.
Elliott, T. & Shadbolt, N. R. (1997). Neurotrophic factors, neuronal selectionism, and neuronal proliferation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):561-562.
Ericsson-Zenith, Steven (forthcoming). Explaining Experience In Nature: The Foundations Of Logic And Apprehension. Institute for Advanced Science & Engineering.
Eronen, Markus I. (2014). Hypothetical identities: Explanatory problems for the explanatory argument. Philosophical Psychology 27 (4):571-582.
Favareau, Donald (2002). Beyond self and other. Sign Systems Studies 30 (1):57-99.
Fekete, Tomer (2010). Representational systems. Minds and Machines 20 (1):69-101.
Fekete, Tomer & Edelman, Shimon (2011). Towards a computational theory of experience. Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):807-827.
Fingelkurts, Andrew A. & Fingelkurts, Alexander A. (2012). Mind as a nested operational architectonics of the brain. Physics of Life Reviews 9 (1):49-50.
Fingelkurts, Andrew A. ; Fingelkurts, Alexander A. ; Bagnato, Sergio ; Boccagni, Cristina & Galardi, Giuseppe (2012). DMN operational synchrony relates to self-consciousness: Evidence from patients in vegetative and minimally conscious states. Open Neuroimaging Journal 6:55-68.
Freeman, Walter J. (1997). Nonlinear neurodynamics of intentionality. Journal of Mind and Behavior 18 (2-3):291-304.
Garson, Justin (2011). Selected effects and causal role functions in the brain: the case for an etiological approach to neuroscience. Biology and Philosophy 26 (4):547-565.
Gaveau, Valérie & Desmurget, Michel (2004). Do movement planning and control represent independent modules? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (1):35-36.
Gentilucci, Maurizio & Chieffi, Sergio (2004). How are cognition and movement control related to each other? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (1):36-37.
Georgiadis, Janniko R. (2012). Doing it . . . wild? On the role of the cerebral cortex in human sexual activity. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology 2.
Gerrans, Philip & Stone, Valerie E. (2008). Generous or parsimonious cognitive architecture? Cognitive neuroscience and theory of mind. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 59 (2):121-141.
Gerrans, Philip (2003). Nativism and neuroconstructivism in the explanation of Williams syndrome. Biology and Philosophy 18 (1):41-52.
Gerrans, Philip (2002). Nativism, neuroconstructivism, and developmental disorder. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):757-758.
Gilman, Daniel (1994). Simplicity, Cognition and Adaptation: Some Remarks on Marr's Theory of Vision. PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1994:454 - 464.
Gold, Ian & Stoljar, Daniel (1999). A neuron doctrine in the philosophy of neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):809-830.
Gold, Ian & Stoljar, Daniel (1999). Interpreting neuroscience and explaining the mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):856-866.
Güell, F. (2014). Pre-dispositional constitution and plastic disposition: toward a more adequate descriptive framework for the notions of habits, learning and plasticity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 (341).
Güell, F. (2014). The liberating dimension of human habit in addiction context. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 (664).
Harbecke, Jens (2011). Mechanistic Constitution in Neurobiological Explanations. International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 24 (3):267-285.
Hardcastle, Valerie Gray (2008). Review of Carl F. Craver, Explaining the Brain: Mechanisms and the Mosaic Unity of Neuroscience. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews 2008 (1).
Hardcastle, Valerie Gray & Stewart, C. Matthew (2001). Theory structure in neuroscience. In Peter K. Machamer, Peter McLaughlin & Rick Grush (eds.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Press.
Harré, Michael S. (2013). From Amateur to Professional: A Neuro-cognitive Model of Categories and Expert Development. Minds and Machines 23 (4):443-472.
Hartmann, Stephan (2001). Mechanisms, coherence, and theory choice in the cognitive neurosciences. In Peter McLaughlin, Peter Machamer & Rick Grush (eds.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. Pittsburgh University Press.
Hartmann, Stephan (2001). Mechanisms, Coherence, and Theory Choice in the Cognitive Neurosciences. In Peter Machamer et al (ed.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences.
Hartmann, Stephan (2001). Mechanisms, coherence, and the place of psychology. In Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. Pittsburgh: University of Pitt Press.
Hatfield, Gary (2010). Review of John Bickle (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Neuroscience. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews 2010 (5).
Heinke, D. (2000). A dynamical system theory approach to cognitive neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):543-543.
Hinton, Geoffrey (2014). Where Do Features Come From? Cognitive Science 38 (6):1078-1101.
Hirstein, (2014). Neuroscience: More Than Just the Latest Paradigm. The Neuropsychotherapist 1:108-109.
Hirstein, William (2010). Loved Ones Near and Far: Feinberg's Personal Significance Theory. Neuropsychoanallysis 12 (2):163-166.
Hirstein, William (2010). The Misidentification Syndromes as Mindreading Disorders. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 (1-3):233-260.
Hurley, Susan L. (2008). The shared circuits model. How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation and mind reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):1-22.
Jacobson, Anne J., Draft: Keeping the World in Mind, Intro & Chpt One.
Johnson, Gregory (2012). The relationship between psychological capacities and neurobiological activities. European Journal for Philosophy of Science 2 (3):453-480.
Kaplan, David Michael & Craver, Carl F. (2011). The Explanatory Force of Dynamical and Mathematical Models in Neuroscience: A Mechanistic Perspective. Philosophy of Science 78 (4):601-627.
Keestra, Machiel (2012). Bounded Mirroring. Joint action and group membership in political theory and cognitive neuroscience. In Frank Vandervalk (ed.), Thinking about the Body Politic: Essays on Neuroscience and Political Theory. Routledge. 222--249.
Keil, Frank, The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations.
Kistler, Max (2009). Cognition and Neurophysiology: Mechanism, Reduction, and Pluralism. Philosophical Psychology 22 (5):539-541.
Klein, Stan (2013). Making the case that episodic recollection is attributable to operations occurring at retrieval rather than to content stored in a dedicated subsystem of long-term memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7 (3):1-14.
Krebs, Peter R., Models of cognition: Neurological possibility does not indicate neurological plausibility.
Kuorikoski, Jaakko & Ylikoski, Petri (2010). Explanatory relevance across disciplinary boundaries: the case of neuroeconomics. Journal of Economic Methodology 17 (2):219–228.
Lane, Timothy & Northoff, Georg (forthcoming). Is depressive rumination rational? In T. W. Hung (ed.), Reason and Rationality. Elsevier.
LaRock, Eric (2007). Disambiguation, binding, and the unity of visual consciousness. Theory and Psychology 17 (6):747-77.
Levy, Arnon (2013). What was Hodgkin and Huxley's Achievement? British Journal for the Philosophy of Science (3):axs043.
Levy, Arnon (2009). Explaining what? Review of explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience by Carl F. Craver. Biology and Philosophy 24 (1).
Machamer et al , Peter (ed.) (2001). Theory and Method in the Neurosciences..
Machamer, Peter K. ; Darden, Lindley & Craver, Carl F. (2000). Thinking about mechanisms. Philosophy of Science 67 (1):1-25.
Machamer, Peter K. ; McLaughlin, Peter & Grush, Rick (eds.) (2001). Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Press.
Machery, Edouard (2012). Dissociations in Neuropsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. Philosophy of Science 79 (4):490-518.
Mashour, George (ed.) (2010). Consciousness, Awareness, and Anesthesia. Cambridge University Press.
Matthen, Mohan (forthcoming). Unique Hues and Colour Experience. Routledge Handbook of the Philosophy of Colour.
Maynard, Patrick (2011). What Drawing Draws On: The Relevance of Current Vision Research. Rivista di Estetica 47 (51):9-29.
McLaughlin, Peter ; Machamer, Peter & Grush, Rick (eds.) (2001). Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. Pittsburgh University Press.
Metzinger, Thomas & Blanke, Olaf (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (1):7-13.
Meurk, Carla ; Carter, Adrian ; Hall, Wayne & Lucke, Jayne (2014). Public Understandings of Addiction: Where do Neurobiological Explanations Fit? Neuroethics 7 (1):51-62.
Meyer, Jean-Arcady & Wilson, Stewart W. (eds.) (1990). From Animals to Animats: Proceedings of The First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior (Complex Adaptive Systems). Cambridge University Press.
Miranda, E. N. (1997). How good are formal neurons for modelling real ones? Acta Biotheoretica 45 (2):171-179.
Miłkowski, Marcin (2013). Explaining the Computational Mind. MIT Press.
Mole, Christopher (2009). On Explaining How Things Work and Explaining How to Work Things. International Journal of Philosophical Studies 17 (5):739-752.
Moskop, John C. (1982). Book Review:Philosophy and Medicine Series. H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr., Stuart F. Spicker; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 1: Explanation and Evaluation in the Biomedical Sciences. H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr., Stuart F. Spicker; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 2: Philosophical Dimensions of the Neuro-Medical Sciences. Stuart F. Spicker, H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr.; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 3: Philosophical Medical Ethics: Its Nature and Significance. Stuart F. Spicker, H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr.; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 4. Mental Health: Philosophical Perspectives. H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr., Stuart F. Spicker; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 5: Mental Illness: Law and Public Policy. Baruch A. Brody, H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr.; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 6: Clinical Judgment: A Critical Appraisal. H. Tristram Engelhardt, Jr., Stuart F. Spicker, Bernard Towers; Philosophy and Medicine Series. Vol. 7. Organism, Medicine, and Metaphysi. Ethics 92 (2):381-.
Mroczko, Aleksandra ; Metzinger, Thomas ; Singer, Wolf & Nikolić, Danko (2009). Immediate transfer of synesthesia to a novel inducer. Journal of Vision 9 (12):1-8.
Mucciolo, Laurence F. (1975). Neurophysiological reduction, psychological explanation and neuropsychology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences 5 (3):451-462.
Mundale, Jennifer & Bechtel, William P. (1996). Integrating neuroscience, psychology, and evolutionary biology through a teleological conception of function. Minds and Machines 6 (4):481-505.
Munsat, Stanley (1999). Neurobiology: Linguistics' millennium bug? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):845-846.
Nagatsu, Michiru (2010). Function and Mechanism: the metaphysics of neuroeconomics. Journal of Economic Methodology 17 (2):197-205.
O'Grady, William (2000). Language, mathematics, and cerebral distinctness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (1):45-45.
O'Meara, J. Tim (1999). Begging the question of causation in a critique of the neuron doctrine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):846-846.
Overgaard, Morten & Jensen, Mads (eds.) (2012). Consciousness and Neural Plasticity. Frontiers Books.
Paton, Bryan ; Skewes, Joshua ; Frith, Chris & Hohwy, Jakob (2013). Skull-bound perception and precision optimization through culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3):222-222.
Patterson, Dennis (2003). Review of M.r. Bennett, P.m.S. Hacker, Philosophical Foundations of Neuroscience. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews 2003 (9).
Pereira Júnior, Alfredo (2001). Coexisting Spatio-Temporal Scales In Neuroscience. Minds and Machines 11 (4):457-465.
Pereira Júnior, Alfredo (2001). Coexisting Spatio-Temporal Scales In Neuroscience. Minds and Machines 11 (4):457-465.
Perone, Sammy & Spencer, John P. (2013). Autonomy in Action: Linking the Act of Looking to Memory Formation in Infancy via Dynamic Neural Fields. Cognitive Science 37 (1):1-60.
Perring, Christian (1999). The neuron doctrine in psychiatry. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):846-847.
Pezzullo, Luca (2002). Cheating neuropsychologists: A study of cognitive processes involved in scientific anomalies resolution. Mind and Society 3 (1):43-50.
Piccinini, Gualtiero (2007). Computational explanation and mechanistic explanation of mind. In Francesco Ferretti, Massimo Marraffa & Mario De Caro (eds.), Cartographies of the Mind: The Interface Between Philosophy and Cognitive Science. Springer. 343-353.
Piccinini, Gualtiero (2006). Computational explanation in neuroscience. Synthese 153 (3):343-353.
Piccinini, Gualtiero & Craver, Carl (2011). Integrating psychology and neuroscience: functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese 183 (3):283-311.
Poirier, Pierre & Faucher, Luc (eds.) (2008). Des Neurones a La Philosophie: Neurophilosophie Et Philosophie Des Neurosciences. Éditions Syllepse.
Poland, Jeffrey S. & Von Eckardt, Barbara (2004). Mechanism and explanation in cognitive neuroscience. Philosophy of Science 71 (5):972-984.
Posner, Michael I. & DiGirolamo, Gregory J. (1999). Flexible neural circuitry in word processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (2):299-300.
Posner, Michael I. ; DiGirolamo, Gregory J. & Fernandez-Duque, Diego (1997). Brain Mechanisms of Cognitive Skills. Consciousness and Cognition 6 (2-3):267-290.
Preston, John (2008). Cognition and the brain: The philosophy and neuroscience movement - edited by Andrew Brook and Kathleen Akins. Philosophical Books 49 (1):68-71.
Quartz, Steven ; Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne ; Machamer, Peter & Scarantino, Andrea, Session 5: Development, neuroscience and evolutionary psychology.
Rathkopf, Charles (2013). Localization and Intrinsic Function. Philosophy of Science 80 (1):1-21.
Revonsuo, Antti (2001). On the nature of explanation in the neurosciences. In Peter K. Machamer, Peter McLaughlin & Rick Grush (eds.), Theory and Method in the Neurosciences. University of Pittsburgh Wpress. 45--69.
Revonsuo, Antti (1999). Neuroscience and the explanation of psychological phenomena. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):847-849.
Rizzolatti, Giacomo & Sinigaglia, Corrado (2007). Mirrors in the Brain: How our minds share actions and emotions. OUP Oxford.
Robinson, Daniel N. (2004). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience by M. R. Bennett and P. M. S. Hacker oxford: Blackwell publishing; 2003. XVII +461pp. Philosophy 79 (1):141-146.
Rosenberg, Alex, Lessons for cognitive science from neurogenomics.
Ryder, Dan (2004). Review essay: Meditations on first neuroscience: Critical notice of mark Changizi's the brain from 25,000 feet. Synthese 141 (2):277 - 285.
Salatino, Dante Roberto (ed.) (2013). Psiquis - Estructura y Función. self publishing.
Schaffner, Kenneth F. (2008). Theories, models, and equations in biology: The heuristic search for emergent simplifications in neurobiology. Philosophy of Science 75 (5):1008-1021.
Scheibel, Arnold B. (1997). The right way, the wrong way, and the army way: A dendritic parable. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):575-575.
Schmaus, Warren (2005). Evolutionary and Neuroscience Approaches to the Study of Cognition. Philosophy of Science 72 (5):675-686.
Schore, Allan N. (2005). Developmental affective neuroscience describes mechanisms at the core of dynamic systems theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (2):217-218.
Shea, Nicholas (2013). Neural mechanisms of decision-making and the personal level. In Kwm Fulford, M. Davies, G. Graham, J. Sadler, G. Stanghellini & T. Thornton (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Psychiatry. OUP. 1063-1082.
Shimono, Masanori & Niki, Kazuhisa (2013). Global mapping of the whole-brain network underlining binocular rivalry. Brain Connectivity 3 (2):212.
Shukla, Vinay K. (2011). Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Neuroethics 4 (3):271-271.
Shultz, Thomas R. & Bale, Alan C. (2006). Neural networks discover a near-identity relation to distinguish simple syntactic forms. Minds and Machines 16 (2):107-139.
Skarda, S. (1986). Explaining behavior: Bringing the brain back in. Inquiry 29 (June):187-201.
Slaney, Kathleen L. & Maraun, Michael D. (2005). Analogy and Metaphor Running Amok: An Examination of the Use of Explanatory Devices in Neuroscience. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 25 (2):153-172.
Slezak, Peter (ed.) (1989). Computers, Brains and Minds. Kluwer.
Soom, Patrice (2012). Mechanisms, determination and the metaphysics of neuroscience. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C 43 (3):655-664.
Sporns, Olaf (2000). Synthetic approaches to cognitive neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):548-549.
Stapp, Henry, Philosophical foundations of neuroscience.
Stinson, Catherine (2009). Searching for the Source of Executive Attention. Psyche 15 (1):137-154.
Stoljar, Daniel & Gold, Ian (1998). On biological and cognitive neuroscience. Mind and Language 13 (1):110-31.
Stone, Tony & Davies, Martin (2000). Autonomous psychology and the moderate neuron doctrine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):849-850.
Stone, Tony & Davies, Martin (1999). Autonomous psychology and the moderate neuron doctrine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):849-850.
Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne (2014). Stabilizing Mental Disorders: Prospects and Problems. In Harold Kincaid & Jacqueline Sullivan (eds.), Classifying Psychopathology: Mental Kinds and Natural Kinds. MIT. 257-281.
Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne (2014). Is the Next Frontier in Neuroscience a Decade of the Mind? In Charles Wolfe (ed.), Brain Theory. Palgrave MacMillan.
Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne (2010). A Role for Representation in Cognitive Neurobiology. Philosophy of Science (Supplement) 77 (5):875-887.
Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne (2009). The multiplicity of experimental protocols: A challenge to reductionist and non-reductionist models of the unity of neuroscience. Synthese 167 (3):511 - 539.
Sullivan, Jacqueline Anne (2008). Memory consolidation, multiple realizations, and modest reductions. Philosophy of Science 75 (5):501-513.
Sutton, John (1999). The churchlands' neuron doctrine: Both cognitive and reductionist. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (5):850-851.
Symons, John (2001). What can neuroscience explain? Brain and Mind 2 (2):243-248.
Tabor, Whitney ; Cho, Pyeong W. & Dankowicz, Harry (2013). Birth of an Abstraction: A Dynamical Systems Account of the Discovery of an Elsewhere Principle in a Category Learning Task. Cognitive Science 37 (7):1193-1227.
Tallis, Raymond C. (2004). Why the Mind Is Not a Computer: A Pocket Lexicon of Neuromythology. Thorverton UK: Imprint Academic.
Teske, John A. (2013). From embodied to extended cognition. Zygon 48 (3):759-787.
Thagard, Paul (ed.) (2007). Philosophy of Psychology and Cognitive Science. North-Holland.
Theurer, Kari & Bickle, John (2013). What's Old Is New Again: Kemeny-Oppenheim Reduction at Work in Current Molecular Neuroscience. Philosophia Scientiae 17 (2):89-113.
Tolbert, Leslie P. ; Oland, Lynne A. ; Christensen, Thomas C. & Goriely, Anita R. (2003). Neuronal and glial morphology in olfactory systems: Significance for information-processing and underlying developmental mechanisms. Brain and Mind 4 (1):27-49.
Toombs, E. (2003). Harmony, explanatory coherence and the debate between the reticular theory and neuron theory of nerve cell structure: Echo's resolution of a quiet revolution. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C 34 (4):615-632.
Tucker, Don M. (2007). Mind From Body: Experience From Neural Structure. Oup Usa.
Tyler, Lorraine K. & Moss, Helen E. (2001). Concepts and categories: What is the evidence for neural specialisation? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (3):495-496.
Vacariu, Gabriel, Unbelievable similarities between Georg Northoff's ideas (Canada, 2011-2014) and Gabriel Vacariu's ideas (2005-2008).
Vacariu, Gabriel (2012). Cognitive neuroscience versus epistemologically different worlds. University of Bucharest Press.
Vacariu, Gabriel (2011). The mind-body problem today. Open Journal of Philosophy 1 (1):26-34.
Weber, Marcel (2008). Causes without mechanisms: Experimental regularities, physical laws, and neuroscientific explanation. Philosophy of Science 75 (5):995-1007.
Weiskopf, Daniel A. (2011). Models and mechanisms in psychological explanation. Synthese 183 (3):313-338.
Werner, M. D. Gerhard (2004). Siren call of metaphor: Subverting the proper task of system neuroscience. [Journal (Paginated)] (in Press).
Wright, Cory D. (2007). Is psychological explanation going extinct? In Huib Looren de Jong & Maurice Schouten (eds.), The Matter of the Mind: Philosophical Essays on Psychology, Neuroscience and Reduction. Oxford: Blackwell.
Wright, Cory D. & Bechtel, William P. (2007). Mechanisms and psychological explanation. In Paul Thagard (ed.), Philosophy of Psychology and Cognitive Science. Elsevier.