Switch to: References

Citations of:

Ethnographic atlas

[Pittsburgh]: University of Pittsburgh Press (1967)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On mechanisms of cultural evolution, and the evolution of language and the common law.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):11-11.
  • Epigenesis and culture.Robert Fagen - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):10-10.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Supernatural beliefs and the evolution of cooperation.Pierrick Bourrat & Hugo Viciana - 2016 - In James Liddle & Todd K. Shackelford (eds.), Oxford Handbook of the Evolutionary Perspectives on Religion. Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cultural evolution and the shaping of cultural diversity.Lesley Newson, Peter Richerson & Robert Boyd - 2007 - Handbook of Cultural Psychology.
    This chapter focuses on the way that cultures change and how cultural diversity is created, maintained and lost. Human culture is the inevitable result of the way our species acquires its behavior. We are extremely social animals and an overwhelming proportion of our behavior is socially learned. The behavior of other animals is largely a product of innate evolved determinants of behavior combined with individual learning. They make quite modest use of social learning while we acquire a massive cultural repertoire (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Evolutionary precursors of social norms in chimpanzees: a new approach.Claudia Rudolf von Rohr, Judith M. Burkart & Carel P. van Schaik - 2011 - Biology and Philosophy 26 (1):1-30.
    Moral behaviour, based on social norms, is commonly regarded as a hallmark of humans. Hitherto, humans are perceived to be the only species possessing social norms and to engage in moral behaviour. There is anecdotal evidence suggesting their presence in chimpanzees, but systematic studies are lacking. Here, we examine the evolution of human social norms and their underlying psychological mechanisms. For this, we distinguish between conventions, cultural social norms and universal social norms. We aim at exploring whether chimpanzees possess evolutionary (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Mate preference is not mate selection.Ada Zohar & Ruth Guttman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):38-39.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A social-cognitive model of human behavior offers a more parsimonious account of emotional expressivity.Vivian Zayas, Joshua A. Tabak, Gul Gunaydy@ 4n, Jeanne M. Robertson & Jacob Miguel Vigil - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (5):407.
    According to socio-relational theory, men and women encountered different ecologies in their evolutionary past, and, as a result of different ancestral selection pressures, they developed different patterns of emotional expressivity that have persisted across cultures and large human evolutionary time scales. We question these assumptions, and propose that social-cognitive models of individual differences more parsimoniously account for sex differences in emotional expressivity.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The place of mind, and the limits of amplification.Joachim F. Wohlwill - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):30-31.
  • Some questions on optimal inbreeding and biologically adaptive culture.George C. Williams - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):116-116.
  • Multilevel selection and the social transmission of behavior.David Sloan Wilson & Kevin M. Kniffin - 1999 - Human Nature 10 (3):291-310.
    Many evolutionary models assume that behaviors are caused directly by genes. An implication is that behavioral uniformity should be found only in groups that are genetically uniform. Yet, the members of human social groups often behave in a uniform fashion, despite the fact that they are genetically diverse. Behavioral uniformity can occur through a variety of psychological mechanisms and social processes, such as imitation, consensus decision making, or the imposition of social norms. We present a series of models in which (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Genes, mind, and culture; A turning point.Thomas Rhys Williams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):29-30.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Culture analyzed in the mode of the natural sciences.Edward O. Wilson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):116-117.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mate selection: Economics and affection.Kim Wallen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):37-38.
  • Gestión cinegética racional ligada a la praxis de la caza y la recolección. ¿Entelequia aristotélica o quimera?Juan Mario Vargas Yáñez, Miguel Ángel Farfán Aguilar, Jesús Duarte Duarte & John E. Fa - 2017 - Arbor 193 (786):415.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resistance to biological self-understanding.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):27-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Male Androphilia in the Ancestral Environment.Doug P. VanderLaan, Zhiyuan Ren & Paul L. Vasey - 2013 - Human Nature 24 (4):375-401.
    The kin selection hypothesis posits that male androphilia (male sexual attraction to adult males) evolved because androphilic males invest more in kin, thereby enhancing inclusive fitness. Increased kin-directed altruism has been repeatedly documented among a population of transgendered androphilic males, but never among androphilic males in other cultures who adopt gender identities as men. Thus, the kin selection hypothesis may be viable if male androphilia was expressed in the transgendered form in the ancestral past. Using the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample (SCCS), (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Information, feedback, and transparency.Robert Van Gulick - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):27-29.
  • Incest, genes, and culture.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):117-123.
  • Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):91-102.
    Much clinical and ethnographic evidence suggests that humans, like many other organisms, are selected to avoid close inbreeding because of the fitness costs of inbreeding depression. The proximate mechanism of human inbreeding avoidance seems to be precultural, and to involve the interaction of genetic predispositions and environmental conditions. As first suggested by E. Westermarck, and supported by evidence from Israeli kibbutzim, Chinese sim-pua marriage, and much convergent ethnographic and clinical evidence, humans negatively imprint on intimate associates during a critical period (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   153 citations  
  • Rethinking innovative designs to further test parasite-stress theory.Ayse K. Uskul - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (2):93-94.
    Fincher & Thornhill's (F&T's) parasite-stress theory of sociality is supported largely by correlational evidence; its persuasiveness would increase significantly via lab and natural experiments and demonstrations of its mediating role. How the theory is linked to other approaches to group differences in psychological differences and to production and dissemination of cultural ideas and practices, need further clarification. So does the theory's view on the possible reduction of negative group interactions.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The innate versus the manifest: How universal does universal have to be?John Tooby & Leda Cosmides - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):36-37.
  • Stability and variation in human evolution.Lionel Tiger - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):115-116.
  • Characteristics of female desirability: Facultative standards of beauty.Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):35-36.
  • The psychology of human mate preferences.Donald Symons - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):34-35.
  • Coalitional Play Fighting and the Evolution of Coalitional Intergroup Aggression.Michelle Scalise Sugiyama, Marcela Mendoza, Frances White & Lawrence Sugiyama - 2018 - Human Nature 29 (3):219-244.
    Dyadic play fighting occurs in many species, but only humans are known to engage in coalitional play fighting. Dyadic play fighting is hypothesized to build motor skills involved in actual dyadic fighting; thus, coalitional play fighting may build skills involved in actual coalitional fighting, operationalized as forager lethal raiding. If human psychology includes a motivational component that encourages engagement in this type of play, evidence of this play in forager societies is necessary to determine that it is not an artifact (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A survey of non-classical polyandry.Katherine E. Starkweather & Raymond Hames - 2012 - Human Nature 23 (2):149-172.
    We have identified a sample of 53 societies outside of the classical Himalayan and Marquesean area that permit polyandrous unions. Our goal is to broadly describe the demographic, social, marital, and economic characteristics of these societies and to evaluate some hypotheses of the causes of polyandry. We demonstrate that although polyandry is rare it is not as rare as commonly believed, is found worldwide, and is most common in egalitarian societies. We also argue that polyandry likely existed during early human (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary origins of patriarchy.Barbara Smuts - 1995 - Human Nature 6 (1):1-32.
    This article argues that feminist analyses of patriarchy should be expanded to address the evolutionary basis of male motivation to control female sexuality. Evidence from other primates of male sexual coercion and female resistance to it indicates that the sexual conflicts of interest that underlie patriarchy predate the emergence of the human species. Humans, however, exhibit more extensive male dominance and male control of female sexuality than is shown by most other primates. Six hypotheses are proposed to explain how, over (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Behavior depends on context.Robert W. Smuts - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):33-34.
  • What are the mechanisms of coevolution?Peter K. Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):114-115.
  • Mind and the linkage between genes and culture.John Maynard Smith - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):20-21.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A bully pulpit.L. B. Slobodkin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):26-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resource Stress Predicts Changes in Religious Belief and Increases in Sharing Behavior.Ian Skoggard, Carol R. Ember, Emily Pitek, Joshua Conrad Jackson & Christina Carolus - 2020 - Human Nature 31 (3):249-271.
    We examine and test alternative models for explaining the relationships between resource stress, beliefs that gods and spirits influence weather, and customary beyond-household sharing behavior. Our model, the resource stress model, suggests that resource stress affects both sharing as well as conceptions of gods’ involvement with weather, but these supernatural beliefs play no role in explaining sharing. An alternative model, the moralizing high god model, suggests that the relationship between resource stress and sharing is at least partially mediated by religious (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Altered States of Consciousness and Psychotherapy A Cross-Cultural Perspective.Mario Simoes - 2002 - International Journal of Transpersonal Studies 21 (1):145-152.
  • The Rebirth of Kinship.Mary K. Shenk & Siobhán M. Mattison - 2011 - Human Nature 22 (1-2):1-15.
    Kinship was one of the key areas of research interest among anthropologists in the nineteenth century, one of the most hotly debated areas of theory in the early and mid-twentieth century, and yet an area of waning interest by the end of the twentieth century. Since then, the study of kinship has experienced a revitalization, with concomitant disputes over how best to proceed. This special issue brings together recent studies of kinship by scientific anthropologists employing evolutionary theory and quantitative methods. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Collaboration between biology and the social sciences: A milestone.Joseph Shepher - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):25-26.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A coup de grace to cultural relativism.Joseph Shepher - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):114-114.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Relationship between subsistence and age at weaning in “preindustrial” societies.Daniel W. Sellen & Diana B. Smay - 2001 - Human Nature 12 (1):47-87.
    Cross-cultural studies have revealed broad quantitative associations between subsistence practice and demographic parameters for preindustrial populations. One explanation is that variationin the availability of suitable weaning foods influenced the frequency and duration of breastfeeding and thus the length of interbirth intervals and the probability of child survival (the “weaning food availability” hypothesis). We examine the available data on weaning age variation in preindustrial populations and report results of a cross-cultural test of the predictions that weaning occurred earlier in agricultural and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Kin and Child Survival in Rural Malawi.Rebecca Sear - 2008 - Human Nature 19 (3):277-293.
    This paper investigates the impact of kin on child survival in a matrilineal society in Malawi. Women usually live in close proximity to their matrilineal kin in this agricultural community, allowing opportunities for helping behavior between matrilineal relatives. However, there is little evidence that matrilineal kin are beneficial to children. On the contrary, child mortality rates appear to be higher in the presence of maternal grandmothers and maternal aunts. These effects are modified by the sex of child and resource ownership: (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Epigenesis: The newer synthesis?Glendon Schubert - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):24-25.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Fitness Costs of Warfare for Women.Michelle Scalise Sugiyama - 2014 - Human Nature 25 (4):476-495.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Co‐occurrence of Ostensive Communication and Generalizable Knowledge in Forager Storytelling.Michelle Scalise Sugiyama - 2021 - Human Nature 32 (1):279-300.
    Teaching is hypothesized to be a species-typical behavior in humans that contributed to the emergence of cumulative culture. Several within-culture studies indicate that foragers depend heavily on social learning to acquire practical skills and knowledge, but it is unknown whether teaching is universal across forager populations. Teaching can be defined ethologically as the modification of behavior by an expert in the presence of a novice, such that the expert incurs a cost and the novice acquires skills/knowledge more efficiently or that (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The Pleasures and Perils of Darwinizing Culture (with Phylogenies).Russell D. Gray, Simon J. Greenhill & Robert M. Ross - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (4):360-375.
    Current debates about “Darwinizing culture” have typically focused on the validity of memetics. In this article we argue that meme-like inheritance is not a necessary requirement for descent with modification. We suggest that an alternative and more productive way of Darwinizing culture can be found in the application of phylogenetic methods. We review recent work on cultural phylogenetics and outline six fundamental questions that can be answered using the power and precision of quantitative phylogenetic methods. However, cultural evolution, like biological (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Is van den Berghe in a new paradigm?Michael Ruse - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):113-114.
  • Homo sociobiologicus not found.R. J. H. Russell & J. Bartrip - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):32-33.
  • Epigenesis and social preference.J. Philippe Rushton - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):31-32.
  • Preference for mates: Cultural choice or natural desire?David C. Rowe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):30-31.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Origins and Maintenance of Female Genital Modification across Africa.Cody T. Ross, Pontus Strimling, Karen Paige Ericksen, Patrik Lindenfors & Monique Borgerhoff Mulder - 2016 - Human Nature 27 (2):173-200.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Are there culturgens?Alexander Rosenberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):22-24.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Permanent Group Membership.Frans L. Roes - 2014 - Biological Theory 9 (3):318-324.
    This article is divided into two main sections. The first discusses “Female Inheritance and the Male Retention Hypothesis.” Permanent groups exist in several species because over generations members share important interests. Considering the association between cooperation and degree of relatedness, it seems to follow that a collective interest is more likely to be achieved when members show a higher degree of relatedness. I argue that if membership is inherited by only one sex, and this is the female sex, this results (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Human sociobiology.Michael J. Reiss - 1984 - Zygon 19 (2):117-140.
    Sociobiology is the scientific study of why organisms sometimes associate with other organisms. This paper surveys recent research on the reasons for altruism and aggression. It also considers the contributions an individual's genes and its environment make to its behavior, and it reviews functional theories for the evolution of cannibalism, polygamy, homosexuality, and infanticide in humans and other animals. Finally mention is made of the limited and generally negative attitude of sociobiology to religion.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations