Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Abstract codes are not just for chimpanzees.Thomas R. Zentall - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):157-158.
  • The pros and cons of having a word for it.S. F. Walker - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):156-157.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Control mechanisms of vocalization and the evolution of speech.Horst D. Steklis - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):287-287.
  • A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
  • Does language training affect the code used by chimpanzees?: Some cautions and reservations.H. L. Roitblat - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):155-156.
  • Doubts about the importance of language training and the abstract code.William A. Roberts - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-155.
  • The codes of man and beasts.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):125-136.
    Exposing the chimpanzee to language training appears to enhance the animal's ability to perform some kinds of tasks but not others. The abilities that are enhanced involve abstract judgment, as in analogical reasoning, matching proportions of physically unlike exemplars, and completing incomplete representations of action. The abilities that do not improve concern the location of items in space and the inferences one might make in attempting to obtain them. Representing items in space and making inferences about them could be done (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • The abstract code as a translation device.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):158-167.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The representational codes for “sameness”.David S. Olton - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-154.
  • Needed: Some specifics for an imaginal code.Richard Millward - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):153-154.
  • Cognition and comparative psychology.George A. Miller - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):152-153.
  • A code by any other name ….Marc Marschark - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):151-152.
  • Why it is unsurprising that ape “language training” enhances “completing incomplete (external) representations of action”.Justin Leiber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):151-151.
  • Problems with Piaget and pallia.Harry J. Jerison - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):284-287.
  • Pragmatism, Neural Plasticity and Mind-Body Unity.Stephen Jarosek - 2013 - Biosemiotics 6 (2):205-230.
    Recent developments in cognitive science provide compelling leads that need to be interpreted and synthesized within the context of semiotic and biosemiotic principles. To this end, we examine the impact of the mind-body unity on the sorts of choices that an organism is predisposed to making from its Umwelt. In multicellular organisms with brains, the relationship that an organism has with its Umwelt impacts on neural plasticity, the functional specialisations that develop within the brain, and its behaviour. Clinical observations, such (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Codes, relations, and mappings.J. Wesley Hutchinson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-149.
  • Tags, alphabets, and the neglect of sound.Stewart H. Hulse - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):148-149.
  • When is a picture worth so many words?Ralph Norman Haber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):147-148.
  • Language training versus training in relations.Lyn Haber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):146-147.
  • Conceptions of development and the evolution of behavior.Gilbert Gottlieb, Timothy D. Johnston & Richard P. Scoville - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):284-284.
  • Brain structure, Piaget, and adaptatison, or, “No, I think, therefore I eat”.Kathleen R. Gibson & Sue T. Parker - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):288-293.
  • The mental representation of action.Jennifer J. Freyd - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):145-146.
  • Homo does not cogitate because of bread alone: Or, “I eat therefore I think?”.Roger S. Fouts - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):283-283.
  • Human cognitive development in the first four years.Kurt W. Fischer - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):282-283.
  • Applying self-directed anticipative learning to science I: Agency, error, and the interactive exploration of possibility space in early ape-langugae research.Robert P. Farrell & C. A. Hooker - 2007 - Perspectives on Science 15 (1):87-124.
    : The purpose of this paper and its sister paper (Farrell and Hooker, b) is to present, evaluate and elaborate a proposed new model for the process of scientific development: self-directed anticipative learning (SDAL). The vehicle for its evaluation is a new analysis of a well-known historical episode: the development of ape-language research. In this first paper we outline five prominent features of SDAL that will need to be realized in applying SDAL to science: 1) interactive exploration of possibility space; (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Applying self-directed anticipative learning to science II: Learning how to learn across a revolution in early ape language research.Robert P. Farrell & C. A. Hooker - 2007 - Perspectives on Science 15 (2):222-255.
    : The purpose of this paper and its sister paper I (Farrell and Hooker, a) is to present, evaluate and elaborate a proposed new model for the process of scientific development: self-directed anticipative learning. The vehicle for its evaluation is a new analysis of a well-known historical episode: the development of ape language research. Paper I examined the basic features of SDAL in relation to the early history of ape-language research. In this second paper we examine the reconceptualization of ape-language (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Effects of language training: Some comparative considerations.Victor H. Denenberg - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):144-145.
  • Tom Sebeok and the external world.John Deely - 2004 - Semiotica 2004 (150):1-21.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Images, labels, concepts, and propositions: Some reservations regarding Premack's “abstract code”.Arthur C. Danto - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):143-144.
  • The replicative evolutionary model of animal and human minds.V. Csånanyi - 1987 - World Futures 23 (3):161-202.
  • Umwelt and Ape Language Experiments: on the Role of Iconicity in the Human-Ape Pidgin Language.Mirko Cerrone - 2018 - Biosemiotics 11 (1):41-63.
    Several language experiments have been carried out on apes and other animals aiming to narrow down the presumed qualitative gap that separates humans from other animals. These experiments, however, have been driven by the understanding of language as a purely symbolic sign system, often connected to a profound disinterest for language use in real situations and a propensity to perceive grammatical and syntactic information as the only fundamental aspects of human language. For these reasons, the language taught to apes tends (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Resemblance and imaginal representation.Ned Block - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):142-143.
  • Social is Emotional.Mette Miriam Rakel Böll - 2008 - Biosemiotics 1 (3):329-345.
    This is a biological approach to the philosophy of mind that feeds an investigation of the phenomena of “social” and “emotional”, both of which are widespread in nature. I scrutinize the non-dualistic Darwinian concept of the continuity of mind. For practical reasons, I address mind at different levels of organization: The systemic mind are the properties of which a common, coherent evolution works upon. Separated from this is “language-mind”: the crystallization of thought in words, which is a strictly human phenomenon. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The last of Clever Hans?Derek Bickerton - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):141-142.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intellectual codes.Jonathan Bennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):139-141.
  • Decomposing intentionality: Perspectives on intentionality drawn from language research with two species of chimpanzees. [REVIEW]William Bechtel - 1993 - Biology and Philosophy 8 (1):1-32.
    In philosophy the term intentionality refers to the feature possessed by mental states of beingabout things others than themselves. A serious question has been how to explain the intentionality of mental states. This paper starts with linguistic representations, and explores how an organism might use linguistic symbols to represent other things. Two research projects of Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, one explicity teaching twopan troglodytes to use lexigrams intentionally, and the other exploring the ability of several members ofpan paniscus to learn lexigram use (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Can you decode a code?Donald M. Baer - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):138-139.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Uexküll, Peirce, and Other Affinities Between Biosemiotics and Biolinguistics.Prisca Augustyn - 2009 - Biosemiotics 2 (1):1-17.
    The purpose of this paper is to describe some parallels and theoretical affinities between biosemiotics and biolinguistics. In particular, this paper examines the importance of Uexküll's Umwelt and Peircean abduction as foundational concepts for Sebeok's biosemiotics and Chomsky's biolinguistic program. Other affinities touched upon in this paper include references to concepts articulated by Immanuel Kant, Konrad Lorenz, Marcel Florkin, François Jacob, C.H. Waddington, D'Arcy Thomson and Ernst Haeckel. While both programs share theoretical influences and historiographical parallels in their mid-century origins (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Animal Studies in the Language Sciences.Prisca Augustyn - 2018 - Biosemiotics 11 (1):121-138.
    This paper explains how recent changes in the ways we study other animals to better understand the human faculty of language are indicative of changing narratives concerning the intelligence of other animals. Uexküll’s concept of Umwelt as a species-specific model of the world is essential to understanding the semiotic abilities of all organisms, including humans. From this follows the view that human language is primarily a cognitive tool for making models of the world. This view is consistent with the basic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representation without process?John R. Anderson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):137-138.