Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Addiction requires philosophical explanation, not mere redescription.Christian Perring - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):592-593.
    Heyman's model explains the irrationality of addictive behavior, but it does not satisfactorily answer the question of whether this behavior is voluntary because it does not address the issue of the choice of preference functions. Furthermore, although Heyman disconfirms the disease model of addiction, this does not resolve the issue of whether addiction should be classified as a mental illness.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Madness and clarity.Adam Wolkin & Robert Cancro - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):225-226.
  • A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction.Roy A. Wise & Michael A. Bozarth - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):469-492.
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
  • Classical conditioning: A manifestation of Bayesian neural learning.James Christopher Westland & Manfred Kochen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-160.
  • A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications.S. F. Walker - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):17-38.
    Critics of the computational connectionism of the last decade suggest that it shares undesirable features with earlier empiricist or associationist approaches, and with behaviourist theories of learning. To assess the accuracy of this charge the works of earlier writers are examined for the presence of such features, and brief accounts of those found are given for Herbert Spencer, William James and the learning theorists Thorndike, Pavlov and Hull. The idea that cognition depends on associative connections among large networks of neurons (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Heyman's steady-state theory of addiction.Stuart A. Vyse - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):598-599.
    Heyman's target article contributes to our understanding of addictions by offering solutions to several paradoxes and by recognizing the stable nature of addictive behavior. Previous classical and operant conditioning models have emphasized molecular processes, such as acquisition and extinction, and have failed to address the aggregate effects of long-term exposure to the contingencies of drug and alcohol use.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Addiction as choice? Yes. As melioration? Maybe, maybe not.Rudy E. Vuchinich - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):597-598.
    Melioration is not superior and may be inferior to other extensions of behavioral choice theory to addiction studies. Progress in the addiction field will be accelerated by the demise of the disease model rather than by attempting to resolve the contradictions it has created.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning beyond the reflex: An uneasy rebirth.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):161-179.
  • Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning is conducted and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • Dopamine and mental illness: And what about the mesocortical dopamine system?J. P. Tassin - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):224-225.
  • Matching observation to addiction theory.Robert M. Swift - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):596-597.
    Over the years, many theories have been proposed to account for the aberrant behavior of drug dependent individuals. Heyman posits that the existing theories of drug dependence are inadequate to explain the complex processes inherent in human drug-taking. He proposes that incongruous behaviors that comprise addiction, such as continued drug use in spite of adverse consequences, can be explained by application of the matching law approach. While the matching law theory of addiction explains certain aspects of human behavior, its application (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward a unified neuropsychiatric hypothesis.Neal R. Swerdlow & George F. Koob - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):226-245.
  • Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania, and depression: Toward a unified hypothesis of cortico-striatopallido-thalamic function.Neal R. Swerdlow & George F. Koob - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):197-208.
  • Psychopharmacology of psychosis: Still looking for missing links.Janice R. Stevens - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):223-224.
  • Neuropsychiatry: Pitfalls of inferring functional mechanisms from observed drug effects.Philippe Soubrié & Pascale Carnoy - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):222-223.
  • The conditioned response: More than a knee-jerk in the ontogeny of behavior.William P. Smotherman & Scott R. Robinson - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):159-160.
  • The Janus faces of addiction.Peter Shizgal - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):595-596.
    Heyman proposes that external stimuli can promote a switch from a local to a global frame of reference for evaluating the consequences of behavior and that such a change might be critical to breaking the grip of drag addiction. Could incentive stimuli promote a switch in the opposite direction and thus contribute to relapse in the recovered addict?
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The contradiction unresolved.Thomas C. Schelling - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):595-595.
    Extreme sensations – thirst, pain – can focus attention on local consequences at the expense of the overall, perhaps for good evolutionary reasons. Maybe the same phenomenon evolves from prolonged use of addictive substances. The matching law explains mistaken choice, not how a person who has confronted personal catastrophe manages to ignore it in making a locally induced choice.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning and language: The old hegemony.Vincent J. Samar & Gerald P. Berent - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):158-159.
  • The pursuit of value: sensitization or tolerance?Terry E. Robinson & Kent C. Berridge - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):594-595.
    Two issues are raised. (1) What is the nature of the drug effect Heyman thinks confers value to drugs? (2) What is the evidence that drug use decreases the value of drugs and of conventional incentives over the long-term? There is considerable evidence for the opposite; a persistent increase in the sensitivity of neural systems that mediate drug value.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: A parsimonious analysis?Anthony L. Riley - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):157-158.
  • In search of the relevant behavioral variables.Joseph J. Plaud - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):593-594.
    Heyman s analysis of the relevant complex behavioral variables associated with addiction is evaluated in relation to identifying the appropriate variables in behavior analysis. The model of behavioral allocation and choice known as melioration, discussed by Heyman as a way to understand the complexities of addiction, is examined and contrasted with another model of matching called ratio invariance, which is offered in this commentary as another behavioral account with a significant potential for resolving the contradictions of addiction.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified theories of psychoses and affective disorders: Are they feasible without accurate neural models of cognition and emotion?Anthony Phillips - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):222-222.
  • Mis-representations.J. Bruce Overmier - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-157.
  • Addiction is not as puzzling as it seems.Jim Orford - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):591-592.
    Heyman's target article seeks to resolve the apparent paradox surrounding the issue of control in relation to addictive behaviour. The present commentary argues that addiction is in fact less paradoxical and more easily understandable than Heyman supposes. A fully satisfactory model of addiction, however, requires a more multifaceted approach than that provided by the type of behavioural choice theory favoured by Heyman.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stimulus factors in addiction.John A. Nevin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):590-591.
    Heyman's analysis of addiction in terms of matching to local relative value can be supplemented by stimulus-control processes. Stimulus equivalence can broaden the set of situations that occasion addictive behavior, and the situation-reinforcer correlation can enhance its persistence. The joint effects of stimulus-control and reinforcement processes may complicate treatment.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebro-cerebellar learning loops and language skills.John W. Moore - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-156.
  • Maximization should sometimes lead to abstinence.Suzanne H. Mitchell & William M. Baum - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):589-590.
    Heyman's model, paradoxically, predicts that whereas a maximizing approach to drug choice will prevent escalation of drug use it will never yield complete abstinence. We suggest an alternative model that overcomes this difficulty by focusing on changes in drug tolerance. A small modification allows maximization to predict either abstinence or moderation (e.g., social drinking).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: The new hyperbole.Ralph R. Miller - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):155-156.
  • Relationship between melioration and the controlling variables.Richard A. Meisch & Ralph Spiga - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):588-589.
    Knowledge of specific instances of melioration does not imply knowledge of the variables controlling behavior. Drug self-administration has been successfully analyzed in terms of standard variables such as the immediacy and magnitude of the drug reinforcer. An appeal to melioration may not be necessary.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Positive reinforcement, the matching law and morality.William A. McKim - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):587-588.
    Addictive behavior has never seemed rational because it persists in spite of drastic aversive consequences. This is a particular problem for models of addiction such as operant psychology which hold that behavior is controlled by its consequences. Inspite of claims to the contrary, Heymans target article illustrates how operant psychology resolves this contradiction. By using the matching law, Heyman suggests a mechanism that explains why delayed aversive events may not control behavior, and a conceptual framework in which we can understand (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Who is at risk for addiction?Dennis McFarland - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):587-587.
    Numerous individual differences determine the effectiveness of the multiple external reinforcement contingencies that control drug seeking behavior. Differences in the capacity to frame reinforcement and also the toxic effects of drugs modulate susceptibility to addiction. As a result, not all individuals are at the same degree of risk for addiction.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding addiction: Conventional rewards and lack of control.Clark McCauley - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):585-586.
    The conflict between drug and conventional rewards leads to a paradox: Sanctions against drug use decrease access to conventional rewards and push drug users toward drug abuse, whereas increased access to the rewards of family, friends, and work may help reduce drug abuse. Lack of control is not specific to drug addiction and is unlikely to yield to a shift in bookkeeping.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is melioration the addiction theory of choice?Robert J. MacCoun - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):586-587.
    Heyman makes a convincing case that a melioration choice strategy is sufficient to produce addictive behavior. But given a plethora of addiction theories, the question is whether melioration theory is superior to rivals more sophisticated than a simple disease model or operant conditioning account. Heyman offers little direct evidence that melioration actually causes the addictions we observe.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Contiguity, contingency, adaptiveness, and controls.Glenda MacQueen, James MacRae & Shepard Siegel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):154-155.
  • Self-control and impulsiveness: Resolution of apparent contradictions in choice behavior.A. W. Logue - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):584-585.
    Choosing the smaller, less delayed reinforcers of drug abuse (including cessation of withdrawal) over the larger, more delayed reinforcers of a healthy life can be described as impulsiveness (the opposite of self-control). Examination of drug abuse using a self-control/impulsiveness framework can help explain many of the puzzles about this behavior, while tying drug abuse into a large research database.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The dark side of hegemony.Charles Locurto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):153-154.
  • Extending the “new hegemony” of classical conditioning.Dan Lloyd - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-153.
  • Toward a neurological psychiatry.Michel Le Moal - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):221-222.
  • An economic perspective on addiction and matching.David I. Laibson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):583-584.
    Economic models of addiction are choice-based. These models push the choice paradigm too far by modeling addiction as rational, normative behavior. Heyman's target article provides a sensible alternative to this economic approach by emphasizing that addiction is characterized by ambivalence and a perceived loss of self control. However, matching may not be a satisfactory platform on which to build this alternative model. Matching experiments do not provide evidence of ambivalence or perceived loss of self control.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning beyond the laboratory.Hugh Lacey - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-152.
  • Pavlovian conditioning: Providing a bridge between cognition and biology.Marvin D. Krank - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):151-151.
  • Searching for a technology of behavior.Bryan Kolb, W. J. Jacobs & Bruce Petrie - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):220-221.
  • Beyond respondent conditioning.Sibylle Klosterhalfen & Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):149-150.
  • A promising new strategy for studying conditioned Immunomodulation.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):150-150.
  • Future directions for the melioration model of addiction.Kris N. Kirby - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):583-583.
    For use in applied settings, the melioration model will need to incorporate changes in the shapes of local value functions over time, treat current value as a continuous function of time since previous choices, and take into account discounting of the effects of current behavior on future value. The policy implications of the model for regulating drugs are limited.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
  • Complexity at the organismic and neuronal levels.R. W. Kentridge - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-148.
  • Dopamine and mental illness: Phenomenological and anatomical considerations.Ann E. Kelley - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):219-220.