Skip to main content
Log in

It’s in your nature: a pluralistic folk psychology

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

I suggest a pluralistic account of folk psychology according to which not all predictions or explanations rely on the attribution of mental states, and not all intentional actions are explained by mental states. This view of folk psychology is supported by research in developmental and social psychology. It is well known that people use personality traits to predict behavior. I argue that trait attribution is not shorthand for mental state attributions, since traits are not identical to beliefs or desires, and an understanding of belief or desire is not necessary for using trait attributions. In addition, we sometimes predict and explain behavior through appeal to personality traits that the target wouldn’t endorse, and so could not serve as the target’s reasons. I conclude by suggesting that our folk psychology includes the notion that some behavior is explained by personality traits—who the person is—rather than by beliefs and desires—what the person thinks. Consequences of this view for the debate between simulation theory and theory theory, as well as the debate on chimpanzee theory of mind are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrews K. (2005). Chimpanzee theory of mind: Looking in all the wrong places? Mind and Language 20: 521–536

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews K. (2007). Critter psychology: On the possibility of nonhuman animal folk psychology. In: Hutto D., Ratcliffe M. (eds) Folk Psychology Reassessed. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly I.A., Robinson E.J. (2003). When can children handle referential opacity? Evidence for systematic variation in 5- and 6-year old children’s reasoning about beliefs and belief reports. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 85: 297–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly I.A., Robinson E.J. (1998). Children’s mental representation of referential relations. Cognition 67: 287–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch K., Wellman H. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton I., Beeghley M. (1982). Talking about internal states. Developmental Psychology 18: 906–921

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain K.M., Heyman G.D., Walker M.E. (1997). Preschoolers’ ability to make dispositional predictions within and across domains. Social Development 6: 53–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell W.K., Sedikides C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology 3: 23–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey S. (1985). Are children fundamentally different kinds of thinkers and learners than adults? In: Chipman S., Segal J., Glaser R. (eds) Thinking and learning skills (vol. 2) Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 485–517

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland P. (1981). Eliminative materialism and the propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy 78: 67–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darley J., Batson C. (1973). From Jerusalem to Jericho: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 27: 100–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson D. (1963). Actions, reasons, and causes. Journal of Philosophy 60: 685–700

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn J., Bretherton I., Munn P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology 23: 132–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan O. (1984). Science of the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Franzoi S.L. (2003). Social psychology. McGraw Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S. (2006). The narrative alternative to theory of mind. In: Menary R. (eds) Radical enactivism: Intentionality, phenomenology, and narrative. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman S.A. (1988). The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories. Cognitive Psychology 20: 65–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P. (2005). Folk psychology as a model. Philosophers’ Imprint 5(6): 1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A. (1989). Interpretation psychologized. Mind and Language 7:L 161–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A. (2006). Simulating minds. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon R. (1995). Folk psychology as simulation. In: Davies M., Stone T. (eds) Folk psychology. Blackwell Publishers, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray C. (2000). Writing social stories with Carol Grey. Future Horizons, Arlington, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller K.A., Berndt T.J. (1981). Developmental changes in the formation and organization of personality attributions. Child Development 52: 683–691

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyman G.D., Gelman S.A. (1999). The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences. Child Development 70: 604–619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hursthouse R. (1991). Arational actions. Journal of Philosophy 88: 57–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto D.D. (2004). The limits of spectatorial folk psychology. Mind and Language 19: 548–573

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd C.M., Ryan C.S., Park B. (1991). Accuracy in the judgment of in-group and out-group variability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61: 366–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish C.W. (2002). Children’s predictions of consistency in people’s actions. Cognition 84: 237–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knobe J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis 63: 190–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda Z. (2002). Social cognition: Making sense of people. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons W. (2001). Matters of the mind. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Malle B.F. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning and social interaction. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Malle B.F. (2006). The relation between judgments of intentionality and morality. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6: 61–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67: 371–378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller J.G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46: 961–978

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller P.H., Aloise P.A. (1989). Young children’s understanding of the psychological causes of behavior: A review. Child Development 60: 257–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols S., Stich S. (2003). Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett R.E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently  and why. The Free Press, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson J.M., Roese N.J., Zanna M.P. (1996). Expectancies. In: Higins E.T., Kruglanski A.W. (eds) Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. Guilford Press, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D. (2004). Small talk and stories: Children’s developing models of the self and others. Cultural Biology: Evolution, development, and mind conference. Waterloo, ON: University of Waterloo, May 21–22.

  • Park B. (1986). A method for studying the development of impressions of real people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51: 907–917

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington N., Hastie R. (1992). Explaining the evidence: Tests of the story model for juror decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16: 90–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rholes W., Newman L.S., Ruble D.N. (1990). Understanding self and other: Developmental and motivational aspects of perceiving persons in terms of invariant dispositions. In: Higgins E.T., Sorrentino R.M. (eds) Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (vol. 2). Guilford Press, New York, pp 369–407

    Google Scholar 

  • Rholes W., Ruble D.N. (1984). Children’s impressions of other persons: The effects of temporal separation of behavioral information. Child Development 57: 872–878

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In: Berkowitz L. (eds) Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 10). Academic Press, New York, pp 174–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology. Temple University Press.

  • Thagard P. (1989). Explanatory coherence. Behavioral and Brian Sciences 12: 435–467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thagard P., Kunda Z. (1997). Making sense of people: Coherence mechanisms. In: Read S., Miller L.C. (eds) Connectionist models of social reasoning and social behavior. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallone R.P., Griffin D.W., Lin S., Ross L. (1990). Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by the self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58: 582–592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H., Cross D., Watson J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72: 655–684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H., Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13: 103–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter L., Uleman J. (1984). When are social judgments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47: 237–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill N., Pearson A. (1998). The development of bases for trait attribution: Children’s understanding of traits as causal mechanisms based on desire. Developmental Psychology 34(3): 574–586

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristin Andrews.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Andrews, K. It’s in your nature: a pluralistic folk psychology. Synthese 165, 13–29 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-007-9230-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-007-9230-5

Keywords

Navigation