Skip to main content
Log in

Is neuroeconomics doomed by the reverse inference fallacy?

  • Published:
Mind & Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Neuroeconomic studies are liable to fall into the reverse inference fallacy, a form of affirmation of the consequent. More generally neuroeconomics relies on two problematic steps, namely the inference from brain activities to the engagement of cognitive processes in experimental tasks, and the presupposition that such inferred cognitive processes are relevant to economic theorizing. The first step only constitutes the reverse inference fallacy proper and ways to correct it include a better sense of the neural response selectivity of the targeted brain areas and a better definition of relevant cognitive ontologies for neuroeconomics. This second way also allows increased coherence between the cognitive processes actually involved in neuroeconomics experiments and the theoretical constructs of economics. We suggest means of increasing neural response selectivity in neuroeconomic experimental paradigms. We also discuss how the choice of cognitive ontologies can both avoid implicit reductionist strategies (from economic constructs to neural patterns) and irrelevance, as cognitive processes engaged in experimental tasks may lack immediate bearing on the study of economic behavior. With these joint improvements neuroeconomics can be a progressive science.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I thank Nicholas Bardsley for having suggested this label and for many comments and improvements on this article.

  2. Magneto-encephalography (MEG) and electro-encephalograhy (EEG) are techniques for mapping brain activity by recording respectively magnetic or electric fields occurring naturally in the brain. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures the change in blood flows in the brain due to neural activity. Near-infrared spectroscopy is an optical method using the correlation between infrared light-waves transmission and metabolic activity in the brain.

  3. As a relevant example one can refer to the ongoing database project brainmap.org, which can compute activation likelihood estimations.

References

  • Bastiaansen J, Thioux M, Keysers C (2009) Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Phil Trans R Soc B 364:2391–2404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bondar I, Leopold D, Richmond D, Victor J, Logothetis N (2009) Long-term stability of visual patterns selective responses of monkey temporal lobe neurons. PLOS One 4:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois-Gironde S, Schoonover C (2008) Cross-talks in economics and neuroscience. Rev Econ Polit 118:35–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Camille N, Coricelli G, Sallet J, Pradat P, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A (2004) The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science 304:1167–1170

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff K, Owen A (2006) Improving reverse neuroimaging inference: cognitive domain versus cognitive complexity. Trends Cogn Sci 10:352–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli G, Nagel R (2009) Neural correlates of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex. PNAS 106:9163–9168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli G, Critchley H, Joffily M, O’Doherty L, Sirigu A, Dolan R (2005) Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nat Neurosci 8:1255–1262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Quervain D, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A, Fehr E (2004) The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305:1254–1258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Cohen L (2008) Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56:384–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsberg D (1961) Risk, ambiguity, and the savage axioms. Q J Econ 75:643–699

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gächter S (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor Jerry (1974) Special sciences: or the disunity of science as a working hypothesis. Synthese 28:97–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gul F, Pesendorfer W (2008) The case for mindless economics. In: Andrew C, Andrew S (eds) The foundations of positive and normative economics. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu M, Ahnen C, Quartz S (2008) The right and the good: distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency. Science 320:1092–1095

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huettel S, Stowe C, Gordon E, Warner B, Platt M (2006) Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron 49(5):765–775

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeankins A, Neil Macrae C, Mitchell J (2008) Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgments of self and others. PNAS 105(11):4507–4512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kable JW, Glimcher PW (2007) The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat Neurosci 10(12):1625–1633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laibson D (1997) Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Q J Econ 112(2):443–477 MIT Press

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loomes G, Sugden R (1982) Regret theory: an alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. Econ J 92:805–824

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2004) Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306(5695):503–507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padoa-Schioppia C, Assad J (2006) Neurons in orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441:223–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce CS (1878) Deduction, induction, and hypothesis. Pop Sci Mon 13:470–482

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack RA (2006) Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):59–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price CJ, Friston KJ (2005) Functional ontologies for cognition: the systematic definition of structure and function. Cogn Neuropsychol 22:262–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey A, Rilling J, Aronson J, Nystrom L, Cohen J (2003) The Neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science 300:1755–1758

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey A, Loewenstein G, McClure S, Cohen J (2006) Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci 30:108–117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer T, Fehr E (2005) The neuroeconomics of mind-reading and empathy. Am Econ Rev 95:340–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sacha Bourgeois-Gironde.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bourgeois-Gironde, S. Is neuroeconomics doomed by the reverse inference fallacy?. Mind Soc 9, 229–249 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-010-0076-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-010-0076-z

Keywords

Navigation