Skip to main content
Log in

Evolutionary Psychology, Meet Developmental Neurobiology: Against Promiscuous Modularity

  • Published:
Brain and Mind

Abstract

Evolutionary psychologists claim that the mind contains “hundreds or thousands” of “genetically specified” modules, which are evolutionary adaptations for their cognitive functions. We argue that, while the adult human mind/brain typically contains a degree of modularization, its “modules” are neither genetically specified nor evolutionary adaptations. Rather, they result from the brain's developmental plasticity, which allows environmental task demands a large role in shaping the brain's information-processing structures. The brain's developmental plasticity is our fundamental psychological adaptation, and the “modules” that result from it are adaptive responses to local conditions, not past evolutionary environments. If different individuals share common environments, however, they may develop similar “modules,” and this process can mimic the development of genetically specified modules in the evolutionary psychologist's sense.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Banton, T., and Bertenthal, B.I., 1997: Multiple developmental pathways for motion processing. Optometry and Visual Science 74, 751–760.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz, A., 1988: The role of gaze compensation of vestibular disfunction: The gaze substitution hypothesis. In O. Pompeiana and J.H.J. (eds), Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 76. Elsevier Science Publishers, pp. 411–420.

  • Brandon, R.N., 1990: Adaptation and Environment. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D.J., 1993: Confirmation and the computational paradigm (or: Why do you think they call it artificial intelligence?). Minds and Machines 3, 155–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D.M., 1995: Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry 6, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P., and Danchin, A., 1976: Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neural networks. Nature 264, 705–712.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S.A., Allard, T., Jenkins, W.M., and Merzenich, M.M., 1988: Receptive fields in the bodysurface map in adult cortex defined by temporally correlated inputs. Nature 332, 444–445.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, W.R., 1995: At War Within: The Double-edged Sword of Immunity. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., 1989: The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31, 187–276.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J., 1987: From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupré(ed.), The Latest on the Best: Essays on Evolution and Optimality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 277–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J., 1994: Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In L.A. Hirschfeld & S.A. Gelman (eds), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 85–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J., 1997: The modular nature of human intelligence. In A.B. Scheibel & J.W. Schopf (eds), The Origin and Evolution of Intelligence, Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett, pp. 71–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, K.S., and Sur, M., 1995: Activity-dependent remodeling of connections in the mammalian visual system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5, 106–111.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T.W., 1997: The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. NewYork: W.W. Norton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Chadarevian, S., 1998: Of worms and programmes: Caenorhabditis elegans and the study of development. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 29(1), 81–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doetsch, G.S., Massey, C.E., Wilson, B.T., and Kelahan, A.M., 1983: Time-dependent functional changes in somatosensory cerebral cortex following digit amputation in adult raccoons. Journal of Comparative Neurology.

  • Dostrovsky, J.O., Millar, J., and Wall, P.D., 1976: The immediate shift of afferent drive of dorsal column nucleus cells following deafferentation: A comparison of acute and chronic deafferentation in gracile nucleus and spinal cord. Experimental Neurology 52, 480–495.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G.M., 1992: right Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J.A., 1986: Natural Selection in the Wild. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., 1983: The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., 2000: The Mind Doesn't Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Futuyma, D.J., 1998: Evolutionary Biology(3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhart, J. and Kirschner, M., 1997: Cells, Embryos, and Evolution. Malden, MA: Blackwell Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gierer, A., and Muller, C.M., 1995: Development of layers, maps, and modules. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5, 91–97.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P., 1999: Genes and codes: Lessons from the philosophy of mind? In V. G. Hardcastle (ed.), Where Biology Meets Psychology: Philosophical Essays, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 305–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P., 2000: On the theoretical role of “genetic coding. ” Philosophy of Science 67, 26–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Isseroff, M., Schwartz, M., and Bugbee, N., 1983: The neurobiology of child development. In F. Plum and V. Mountcastle (eds), The Handbook of Child Development. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society, pp. 282–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, E., Reeves, A.J., Graziano, M.S.A., and Gross, C.G., 1999: Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science 286, 548–552.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, V.G., 1995: Locating Consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Innocenti, G.M., 1995: Exuberant development of connections, and its possible permissive role in cortical evolution. Trends in Neuroscience 18, 397–402.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L.C., and Shatz, C.J., 1996: Synaptic activity and the construction of cortical circuits. Science 274, 1133–1138.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macalusao, E., Frith, C.D., and Driver, J., 2000: Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. Science 289, 1206–1208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, A.R., Sekuler, A.B., Penpeci, C., Rajah, M.N., Grady, C.L., Sekuler, R., and Bennett, P.J., 1999: Recruitment of unique neural systems to support visual memory in normal aging. Current Biology 9, 1275–1278.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merrill, E.G., and Wall, P.D., 1978: Plasticity of connection in the adult nervous system. In C.W. Cotman (ed.), Neuronal Plasticity. New York: Raven Press, pp. 94–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M.M, Kaas, J.H., Sur, M., Nelson, R.J., and Felleman, D.J., 1983: Progression of change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys. Neuroscience, 639–665.

  • Metzler, J., and Marks, P.S., 1979: Functional changes in cat somatic sensory-motor cortex during short-term reversible epidermal blocks. Brain Research 177, 379–383.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, F.A., and Lisberger, S.G., 1981: Plasticity in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: A new hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 4, 273–299.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Penn, A.A., and Shatz, C.J., 1999: Brain waves and brain wiring: The role of endogenous and sensory-driven neural activity in development. Pediatric Research 45, 447–458.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., 1997: How the Mind Works. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z., 1984: Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V.S., 1994: Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes, repressed memories, and Freudian psychology. International Review of Neurobiology 37, 291–372.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley, M., 1996: Evolution (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, R., 1998: Evolutionary psychology and the massive modularity hypothesis. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 49, 575–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanes, J.N., Donoghue, J.P., Thangaraj, V., Edelman, R.R., and Warach, S., 1995: Shared neural substrates controlling hand movements in human motor cortex. Science 268, 1775–1777.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, P. forthcoming. Realization, reduction and psychological autonomy. Synthese.

  • Shatz, C.J., 1996: Emergence of order in visual system development. Journal of Physiology – Paris 90, 141–150.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sinninger, Y.S., Doyle, K.J., and Moore, J.K., 1999: The case for early identification of hearing loss in children. Auditory system development, experimental auditory deprivation, and development of speech perception and hearing. Pediatric Clinics in North America 46, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E., 1984: The Nature of Selection. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E., 2000: Philosophy of Biology (2nd ed.). Boulder, CO: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D., 1987: If we're all Darwinians, what's the fuss about? In C. Crawford, et al. (eds), Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 121–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D., 1992: On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. In J.H. Barkow, et al. (eds), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 137–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R.F., 1993: The Brain: A Neuroscience Primer. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L., 1992: The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, et al. (eds), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L., 1995: Foreword. In S. Baron-Cohen, Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. xi–xviii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wall, P.D. and Egger, M.D., 1971: Formation of new connections in adult rat brains after partial deafferentation. Nature, New Biology 232, 542–545.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, M.J., Ungerleider, L.G., and Bachevalier, J., 1995: Development and plasticity of the neural circuitry underlying visual recognition memory. Canadian Journal of Physiological Pharmacology 73, 1364–1371.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winberg, J., and Porter, R.H., 1998: Olfaction and human neonatal behavior: Clinical implications. Acta Paediatrica 87, 6–10.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, R.O., 1999: Retinal waves and visual system development. Annual Review in Neuroscience 22, 29–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Buller, D.J., Hardcastle, V. Evolutionary Psychology, Meet Developmental Neurobiology: Against Promiscuous Modularity. Brain and Mind 1, 307–325 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011573226794

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011573226794

Navigation