Elsevier

Consciousness and Cognition

Volume 20, Issue 4, December 2011, Pages 1265-1271
Consciousness and Cognition

Subliminal spatial cues capture attention and strengthen between-object link

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.007Get rights and content

Abstract

According to the spreading hypothesis of object-based attention, a subliminal cue that can successfully capture attention to a location within an object should also cause attention to spread throughout the whole cued object and lead to the same-object advantage. Instead, we propose that a subliminal cue favors shifts of attention between objects and strengthens the between-object link, which is coded primarily within the dorsal pathway that governs the visual guidance of action. By adopting the two-rectangle method and using an effective subliminal cue to compare with the classic suprathreshold cue, we found a different result pattern with suprathreshold cues than with subliminal cues. The suprathreshold cue replicated the conventional location and object effects, whereas a subliminal cue led to a different-object advantage with a facilitatory location effect and a same-object advantage with an inhibitory location effect. These results support our consciousness-dependent shifting hypothesis but not the spreading hypothesis.

Highlights

► We hypothesize subliminal spatial cues trigger vision-for-action (dorsal) pathway. ► Subliminal cues favor between-object shift and strengthens between-object link. ► We obtain opposite result patterns with subliminal and suprathreshold cues. ► Results confirm our consciousness-dependent shifting hypothesis. ► Results dispute the spreading hypothesis of object-based attention.

Introduction

To recognize an object in a multi-object scene, our brain needs to calculate the relation of properties—shape, color, configuration, and so on—within objects. For example, a pail with a curvature on the side can be a mug, but if the curvature is on the top of the pail, it is more likely to be a bucket (Biederman, 1987). Visual attention can facilitate processing of properties belonging to the same object, that is, object-based attention (Duncan, 1984, Egly et al., 1994), and this kind of object-based attention may be achieved by strengthening the within-object link that is critical for object recognition. However, to act in a multi-object environment, our brain needs to calculate the relation of properties—orientation, size, and distance—between objects. For example, to hit a baseball, it is critical to know the moment-by-moment distance between the ball and the bat. In this case, it is likely that attention helps action execution by strengthening the between-object link (Davis, 2001, Humphreys, 1998) that is important for visually guided action.

Indeed, two visual pathways have been identified for the two main functions of vision: object recognition and action (Goodale & Milner, 1992). The ventral pathway—from visual primary cortex (V1) to temporal cortex—is mainly involved in object recognition, whereas the dorsal pathway—from V1 to frontal-parietal cortex—is mainly involved in the visual guidance of action (Goodale et al., 1991, Kluver and Bucy, 1938). The double-dissociation demonstrated by neuropsychological patients provides evidence for the two-pathway theory. On one hand, patients with lesion areas in the ventral pathway lose conscious vision for object recognition but not the unconscious vision to act (Goodale & Milner, 2004). For example, Patient DF cannot report the orientation of a pencil, but she can posture her hand correctly as she reaches out to grasp it (Goodale et al., 1991). On the other hand, patients with lesions in the dorsal pathway have intact object recognition but impaired visually guided action. These optic ataxia patients are able to report the orientation of a slot cut in a disk, but they cannot reach out and pass their hand through it (Perenin & Vighetto, 1988). Contrary to Patient DF, the optic ataxia patients have conscious vision for object recognition but they cannot use this vision to guide their action. This double-dissociation of conscious and unconscious vision revealed by neuropsychological patients with damage in ventral and dorsal pathways, respectively, hints at the possibility that manipulating normal participants’ consciousness of the stimuli can dissociate the two pathways and show their difference in affecting performance. This is the goal of the current study.

In a seminal paper, Egly and colleagues (1994) used a cueing paradigm with a double-rectangle display to demonstrate the existence of within-object link. They presented two outlined rectangles, with one end of one rectangle brightened as a cue to indicate the possible location of a target. The target was a small solid square, shown subsequently within one end of a rectangle. Within-object link was indicated by the same-object advantage: RTs were shorter when the target appeared at the uncued end of the cued than at the uncued rectangle, with an equal cue-to-target distance between the two. Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) suggest that there is a cost of shifting attention between objects (see also Lamy & Egeth, 2002). As with Egly et al. (1994), a series of studies showing this same-object advantage have used suprathreshold stimuli that supposedly trigger conscious vision in the ventral pathway (e.g., Abrams and Law, 2000, Lamy and Tsal, 2000, Moore et al., 1998). Here we suggest that vision for action, which is triggered by unconscious vision, may favor shifts of attention between objects relative to shifts of attention within objects. Therefore, we hypothesize that if subliminal stimuli are used, different-object advantage—that is, faster response to a target within an uncued object than within a cued object—should be obtained instead because unconscious vision involved in the dorsal pathway is primarily for action, and action requires a between-object link (cf. Davis, 2001).

In contrast to this consciousness-dependent shifting hypothesis, the influential spreading hypothesis of object-based attention would make an opposite prediction (Richard, Lee, & Vecera, 2008). The spreading hypothesis states that when attention is cued to a location within an object, attention will spread automatically from the cued location to the whole object. Consequently, a subliminal cue that can successfully capture attention to a specific location within an object should also cause attention to spread throughout the whole cued object. In sum, regardless of the participant’s awareness of the cue, a conventional same-object advantage is expected.

To test these two hypotheses, we designed four experiments orthogonally by crossing the cue type (subliminal, suprathreshold) with the cue-to-target stimuli-onset-asynchrony (SOA; 100 ms, 1000 ms). A subliminal cue was followed by suprathreshold cue in each pair of experiments. The experiments were structured as follows:

  • Experiment 1: subliminal cue, 100-ms SOA.

  • Experiment 2: suprathreshold cue, 100-ms SOA.

  • Experiment 3: subliminal cue, 1000-ms SOA.

  • Experiment 4: suprathreshold cue, 1000-ms SOA.

Manipulation of SOA allows us to examine the object effects induced by the spatial cue across different time courses. Past studies using suprathreshold cues have shown that one’s attention is attracted first to the cued location but then is inhibited from going there again, as indicated by an early facilitation (faster RT) followed by late inhibition (slower RT) at the cued location (for a review, see Klein, 2000). Bennett and Pratt (2001) examined the spatial distribution of the late-inhibition component and found facilitation in the quadrant opposite to the cued (inhibited) location. Assuming that attention relocates to the opposite quadrant in the long-SOA condition, the uncued object in the current study becomes “attended-object,” and a reversed object effect should be obtained. Indeed, with suprathrehold cues, results opposite to that obtained in the short SOAs were found instead (Jordan & Tipper, 1999).

A recent study by Mulckhuyse, Talsma, and Theeuwes (2007) has shown the same bi-phasic mode of early facilitation and late inhibition with a subliminal spatial cue. Thus, we hypothesize that opposite object effects should be obtained for a long-SOA condition compared with a short-SOA condition for the subliminal cue as well. That is, with subliminal cues, we should expect to find a different-object advantage for a short SOA and a same-object advantage for a long SOA. In contrast, the spreading hypothesis (Richard et al., 2008) predicts conventional object effects—same-object advantage for a short SOA (Egly et al., 1994) and different-object advantage for a long SOA (Jordan & Tipper, 1999)—as long as a subliminal cue attracts attention to its location.

Section snippets

Participants

Seventy-seven paid volunteers participated in this study (N = 29, 20, 17, and 11 in Experiments 1–4, respectively). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naïve as to the purpose of the experiment.

Stimuli, apparatus, and design

The stimuli were presented on a VGA monitor with the resolution of 640 × 480 pixels in a 256-color mode. A visual C++ computer program was run on an IBM-compatible computer to present the stimuli and collect the RT data. Participants sat in a dimly lit chamber with a viewing

Cue-report task

All participants in Experiments 1 and 3 reported that they were unable to perceive the cue, which was corroborated by the objective measure. The mean detection accuracy of the four-alternative-forced-choice cue-report tasks were not significantly above chance level (27% and 25%, respectively; ps > .30). All participants in Experiments 2 and 4 were fully aware of the cue.

Target-detection task

Fig. 2 shows the mean correct RTs collapsed across rectangle orientation in all experiments, since orientation did not affect

Discussion

Our results show that a subliminal cue caused different-object advantage for short SOA (Experiment 1) and same-object advantage for long SOA (Experiment 3). These results are opposite to the object effects obtained with a suprathreshold cues used in Experiments 2 and 4 in which conventional object effects were replicated: same-object advantage for short SOA (e.g., Abrams and Law, 2000, Egly et al., 1994, Lamy and Tsal, 2000, Moore et al., 1998) and different-object advantage for long SOA (e.g.,

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Taiwan’s National Science Council (NSC 96-2413-H-002-009-MY3 and NSC 98-2410-H-002-023-MY3). We thank Tram Neill, Greg Davis, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

References (31)

  • P.T. Brawn et al.

    Attention to overlapping objects: Detection and discrimination of luminance changes

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (2000)
  • G. Davis

    Between-object links and visual attention

    Visual Cognition

    (2001)
  • G. Davis et al.

    Can attention select only a fixed number of objects at a time?

    Perception

    (2001)
  • J. Duncan

    Selective attention and the organization of visual information

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1984)
  • R. Egly et al.

    Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1994)
  • Cited by (12)

    • Action inhibition and affordances associated with a non-target object: An integrative review

      2020, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similar to the compatibility effect observed with the abstract objects, these cue studies show that when the spatial prime is presented in the same location or side as the upcoming target (i.e., it cues the target location), the perceptual and response processes related to the target are facilitated (Posner, 1988; Posner and Petersen, 1990; Paavilainen et al., 2016). This facilitatory cueing effect occurs in manual responses even when the spatial prime is presented subliminally (Chou and Yeh, 2011). In addition, the spatial (left-right) cues have been observed to produce initial increased motor activation in the side of the motor cortex that is compatible with the cue location in these spatial cueing tasks (e.g., Eimer, 1995; Paavilainen et al., 2016).

    • Dissociating location-based and object-based cue validity effects in object-based attention

      2018, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The same-object advantage is not merely an effect of crossing the boundaries of the rectangles either, because the effect has been demonstrated with occlusion and under various grouping situations (e.g., Behrmann, Zemel, & Mozer, 1998; Lamy & Tsal, 2000; Lin & Yeh, 2012; Moore, Yantis, & Vaughan, 1998; Watson & Kramer, 1999). As with Egly et al. (1994), a series of studies using various stimuli and tasks have demonstrated both the spatial-cueing effect and the same-object advantage (e.g., Avrahami, 1999; Chou & Yeh, 2011; Chou & Yeh, 2012; Ho & Yeh, 2009; Lamy & Tsal, 2000; Lin & Yeh, 2011; Lin & Yeh, 2012; Moore et al., 1998). In the current study, we independently manipulated cue validity based on location and that based on object as a whole in the double-rectangle display.

    • Solving the paradox between same-object advantage and different-object advantage

      2015, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the past 30 years, these two types of SOA effect have been repeatedly demonstrated under various circumstances in numerous studies (Chen, 2012; Scholl, 2001). On the other hand, the different-object advantage (DOA) effect shows a pattern that is exactly opposite to that of the SOA effect (Cepeda & Kramer, 1999; Chou & Yeh, 2011; Davis, 2001; Davis & Holmes, 2005; Davis, Welch, Holmes, & Shepherd, 2001; Harrison & Feldman, 2009). That is, participants’ performance was even worse when judging two stimuli or features within the same object than those in different objects.

    • Spatial distortion induced by imperceptible visual stimuli

      2013, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, Blanco and Soto (2009) reported that the line-motion illusion did not depend on conscious awareness of the flashed cue because it occurred even when the cue was rendered invisible by the presentation of a mask. This technique of masking a very briefly presented cue to suppress the visibility of the stimuli (backward masking) has also been applied to the study of object-based attention: Chou and Yeh (2011) examined whether a subliminal positional cue followed by a mask would affect the reaction time for identifying the position of the target. A subliminal cue facilitated a different-object advantage in locating the target, whereas a supraliminal cue facilitated a same-object advantage.

    • Attention guidance technique using visual subliminal cues and its application on videos

      2021, IMX 2021 - Proceedings of the 2021 ACM International Conference on Interactive Media Experiences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text