Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Volume 12, Issue 9, September 2008, Pages 327-333
Journal home page for Trends in Cognitive Sciences

Opinion
What's next? New evidence for prediction in human vision

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.06.001Get rights and content

Everyday visual experience involves making implicit predictions, as revealed by our surprise when something disturbs our expectations. Many theories of vision have been premised on the central role played by prediction. Yet, implicit prediction in human vision has been difficult to assess in the laboratory, and many results have not distinguished between the indisputably important role of memory and the future-oriented aspect of prediction. Now, a new and unexpected finding – that humans can resume an interrupted visual search much faster than they can start a new search – offers new hope, because the rapid resumption of a search seems to depend on participants forming an implicit prediction of what they will see after the interruption. These findings combined with results of recent neurophysiology studies provide a framework for studying implicit prediction in perception.

Section snippets

What's next? New evidence for prediction in human vision

The next time you hear familiar footsteps outside your door, pay close attention to your visual experience when the person appears. If your prediction is confirmed, you will fluently identify the person and begin your social exchange. But if those footsteps belong to an unexpected person, albeit one familiar to you, you will do a double-take. Your visual identification will be momentarily frozen by the mismatch between your auditory-based prediction and the person's appearance.

Analogs of these

Recent developments in the neurobiology of predictive vision

Despite this ambiguity, there is renewed interest in perceptual prediction, which has been driven by studies of brain imaging in humans and electrophysiology in animals. These studies reveal that the time course of brain activation does not follow the conventional anatomical hierarchy 33, 34. Some higher-level cortical areas receive signals from the eyes at the same time, or even before, some so-called lower-order areas. Other studies show that communication between two brain areas is generally

Unexpected finding offers new evidence of prediction in vision

The recent finding of a unique pattern of search behavior in humans promises a way, from within this predictive-brain framework, to study implicit visual prediction. The main result is that we can resume a previously interrupted search much faster than we can start a new search [49]. The evidence for prediction – versus merely fluent access to memory – is based on two crucial findings. The first finding is the sheer speed with which a search can resume after an interruption. Correct responses

Concluding remarks: the future of implicit perceptual predictions

We have interpreted the finding that humans can resume an interrupted search much faster than they can start a new search 49, 52, 53, 54 as evidence that implicit predictions are formed in the course of everyday visual behavior, such as finding your keys on a cluttered desk. But questions remain (Box 5), and there is much work to be done, both to confirm that the rapid resumption of a search really involves forward-acting perceptual predictions and then to study the nature of those predictions

Acknowledgements

The preparation of this article was supported by a Discovery Grant (NSERC Canada) and a UBC Study Leave (2007–08) to J.T.E. The authors are grateful to Jeremy Wolfe for critical comments in review and especially for suggesting the importance of distinguishing between theories based on prediction versus the simple accumulation of evidence.

References (62)

  • J.M. Wolfe

    How fast can you change your mind? The speed of top-down guidance in visual search

    Vision Res.

    (2004)
  • B.J. Baars et al.

    How conscious experience and working memory interact

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2003)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Consciousness and working memory

    Conscious. Cogn.

    (1992)
  • L. Trick et al.

    Lifespan changes in attention: the visual search task

    Cogn. Dev.

    (1998)
  • L. Iordanescu

    Characteristic sounds facilitate visual search

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (2008)
  • B. Balas et al.

    ‘Filling-in’ colour in natural scenes

    Vis. Cogn.

    (2007)
  • von Helmholtz, H. (1866/1962) Concerning the perceptions in general. In Treatise on Physiological Optics (Vol. 3, 3rd...
  • J. Hochberg

    In the mind's eye

  • R.L. Gregory

    Perceptions as hypotheses

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (1980)
  • I. Rock

    In defense of unconscious inference

  • U. Neisser

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1967)
  • R.J. Bootsma et al.

    Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1993)
  • D.J. Law

    Perceptual and cognitive factors governing performance in comparative arrival time judgments

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1993)
  • J.T. Todd

    Visual information about moving objects

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1981)
  • J.Y. Lin

    Objects on a collision path with the observer demand attention

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2008)
  • A. Lleras et al.

    Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming

    J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.

    (2004)
  • M. Bar et al.

    Subliminal visual priming

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1998)
  • B. Milliken

    Selective attention: a re-evaluation of the implications of negative priming

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1998)
  • A. Torralba

    Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search

    Psychol. Rev.

    (2006)
  • J.M. Henderson

    Covert visual attention and extrafoveal information use during object identification

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1989)
  • J.E. Hoffman et al.

    Saccadic eye movements and visual selective attention

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1995)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text