Skip to main content
Log in

Louise Barrett, beyond the brain: how body and environment shape animal and human minds

Princeton University Press, 2011. 304 pp., ISBN: 9781400838349, $29.95

  • Book Review
  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. Affordances are the terms in which perceptual information is made available.

  2. John Sutton has written a fair bit about how Descartes did allow for this kind of body–brain integration. In Sutton’s view; modern forms of Cartesianism must be intended as deviations from the Magister’s Dictum because they have failed to keep abreast of the spirit of his philosophy. So, Cartesianism would interestingly be not true to the spirit of Descartes! For more specific details, see Sutton (1998) and Gaukroger et al. (2000). Thanks to Julian Kiverstein for the hint.

  3. For a series of interesting studies, see Guthrie 1997, Datson and Mitman 2005, and Horowitz 2007. For more popular treatments, see also Patten 2006 and Myers 2008].

  4. This seems to provide nice empirical evidence for what Mark Rowlands (1999) has called the “barking dog principle.” “If it is necessary for an organism to be able to perform a given adaptive task T, then it is differentially selectively disadvantageous for that organism to develop internal mechanisms sufficient for the performance of T when it is possible for the organism to perform T by way of a combination of internal mechanisms and manipulation of the external environment” (Rowlands 1999), p.80]. This principle is very similar to the 007 principle formulated by Clark in 1989. It is important to note that the 007 principle has been consistently used, in the literature directed at the extended mind thesis, to endorse the case of cognitive extension. For an interesting analysis of the relationships between the two, see Shapiro 2010.

References

  • Barton, R. A. (2006). Primate brain evolution: integrating comparative, neuropsychological and ethological data. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15, 224–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blessing, W. (1997). The lower brainstem and bodily homeostasis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L., Ring, B., & Kling, A. (1992). Response in neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behavioral Brain Research, 41, 199–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. (2006). Why are animals cognitive? Current Biology, 16, R445–R448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Datson, L., & Mitman, G. (2005). Thinking with animals: new perspectives on anthropomorphism. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT2: the language of thought revisited. USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaukroger, S., Schuster, J., Sutton, J. (2000). Descartes’ Natural Philosophy. Routledge.

  • Gauthier, I., Skudlaski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey Walter, W. (1951). A machine that learns. Scientific American, 185, 60–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey Walter, W. (1953). The living brain. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. E. (1997). Anthropomorphism: a definition and a theory. In R. W. Mitchell, N. S. Thompson, & H. L. Miles (Eds.), Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals (pp. 50–58). Albany: State University New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and Time. London: SCM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, O. (2003). The first biologically inspired robot. Robotica, 21, 351–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2007). Anthropomorphism. In M. Bekoff (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human-animal relationships (pp. 60–66). Westport: Greenwood Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialised for the perception of faces. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiverstein, J., & Farina, M. (2011). Integration and the extended mind thesis (Provisional Title) (in press).

  • MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the ‘visceral brain’: recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 11, 338–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. C. (1982). Vision: a computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2007). Cognitive integration: mind and cognition unbounded. New York: Palgrave Macmillian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. New York: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, B. (10 June 2008). “Why we are all animal lovers”. The Guardian. Retrieved 6 May 2010.

  • Patten, F. (2006). Furry! The world's best anthropomorphic fiction. ibooks. pp. 427–436.

  • Perrett, D. I. (1999). A cellular basis for reading minds from faces and actions. In M. Hauser & M. Konishi (Eds.), Behavioral and neural mechanisms of communication (pp. 159–185). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2003). How the mind works. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio, T. (1981). Marr’s computational approach to vision. Trends in Neurosciences, 10, 258–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, P. (2007). Extraordinary animals: an encyclopedia of curious and unusual animals. Westport: Greenwood Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowlands, M. (1999). The body in mind: understanding cognitive processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rowlands, M. (2006). Against methodological solipsism: the ecological approach. Philosophical Psychology, 8(1), 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowlands, M. (2010). The new science of the mind: from extended mind to embodied phenomenology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rupert, R. (2009). Cognitive systems and the extended mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. (2010). James Bond and the barking dog: evolution and extended cognition. Philosophy of Science, 77, 400–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. (1998). Philosophy and memory traces: Descartes to connectionism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: history, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind. (pp. 189–225). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gelder, T. (1995). What might cognition be, if not computation? Journal of Philosophy, 92, 435–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Uexküll, J. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. H. Shiller & K. S. Lashley (Eds.), Instinctive behavior: the development of a modern concept (pp. 5–82). Madison: International Universities Press. Original work published in 1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M. (2005). Reconstructing the cognitive world: the next step. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, S., & Jackson, R. (2002). Jumping spider tricksters. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 27–34). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am deeply indebted to Julian Kiverstein and John Sutton for their continuous guidance and invaluable support throughout my doctoral studies. My warmest gratitude also goes to Andy Clark and Richard Menary for their stimulating feedback on earlier drafts of this review. Thanks also to Joel Krueger for the precious assistance through the editorial process. Last but not least, I would like to express my appreciation to the ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders (CCD) and to Macquarie University for generously financing my research. Needless to say, any remaining errors are mine and mine alone.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mirko Farina.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Farina, M. Louise Barrett, beyond the brain: how body and environment shape animal and human minds. Phenom Cogn Sci 11, 415–421 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9247-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9247-6

Navigation