Skip to main content
Log in

On the Genetic and Epigenetic Bases of Primate Signal Processing

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Four sequential, sub-processes are identified as the fundamental steps in the processing of signals by big-brained animals. These are, Detection of the signal, its Representation in correlated sensory brain structure, the Interpretation of the signal in another part of the brain and the Expression of the receiver’s response. We label this four-step spatiotemporal process DRIE. We support the view that when the context within which such signals are produced and received is relatively constant, the DRIE process can be ultimately assimilated into the genome, with the Interpretation sub-phase is markedly decreased in duration as speed and efficiency are maximized. With frequent repetition and learning, an analogous result can be attained epigenetically as exemplified in human word and text recognition, allowing tasks critical to primate social function to be accomplished with rapidity and accuracy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a fuller examination of the significance of what is meant by spatiotemporality, see Goldberg and Swan (2011). “A Biosemiotic Analysis of Braille.” Biosemiotics 4(1): 25–38.

References

  • Andrews, M. W., Bhat, M. C., et al. (1995). Acquisition and long-term patterning of joystick selection of food-pellet vs social-video reward by bonnet macaques. Learning and Motivation, 26, 370–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M. W., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1993). Live-social-video reward maintains joystick task performance in bonnet macaques. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77(3 Pt 1), 755–763.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M. W., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1994). The development of affiliative and agonistic social patterns in differentially reared monkeys. Child Development, 65(5), 1398–1404.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M. W., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1995). Location effects of social partner on experimental foraging in the squirrel monkey. American Journal of Primatology, 36, 175–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M. W., & Rosenblum, L. A. (2002). Response patterns of bonnet macaques following up to 75 weeks of continuous access to social-video and food rewards. American Journal of Primatology, 57(4), 213–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barbieri, M. (2011). Origin and evolution of the brain. Biosemiotics. doi:10.1007/s12304-011-9125-1.

  • Browne, P. A., & Goldberg, L. J. (1978). The effect of modifying presynaptic input on the discarge patterns of trigeminal interneurons. Brain Research, 148, 234–239.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crompton, A.W. and Jenkins, F.A. (1979). Origin of mammals. Mesozoic mammals: The first two-thirds of mammalian history. In J. A. Lillegraven, Z. Kielan-Jaworowska and W. A. Clemens (ed.), (pp. 59–73). Berkeley: University of California Press.

  • Goldberg, L. J., & Nakamura, Y. (1968). Lingually induced inhibition of masseteric motoneurones. Experientia, 24(4), 371–373.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. J., & Swan, L. S. (2011). A boiosemiotic analysis of Braille. Biosemiotics, 4(1), 25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. J., & Tal, M. (1978). Intracellular recording in trigeminal motoneurons of the anesthetized guinea pig during rhythmic jaw movements. Experimental Neurology, 58(1), 102–110.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (1996). Signs of meaning in the universe. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iberall, A. and McColloch, W.S. (1969). “The organizing principle of complex living systems.” Journal of Basic Engineering June: 290–294.

  • Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92(1–2), 101–144.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, C. I., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1966). A behavioral taxonomy for Macaca nemestrina and Macaca radiata: based on longitudinal observation of family groups in the laboratory. Primates, 7(2), 205–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luschei, E. S., & Goldberg, L. J. (1981). Neural mechanisms of mandibular control: mastication and voluntary biting. Suplement 2: Handbook of physiology, the nervous system, motor control. V. B. Brooks (pp. 1237–1274). Washington, DC: American Physiological Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papineau, D. (2005). Social learning and the Baldwin effect. Evoution, Rationality and Cognition: A Cognitive Science for the Twenty-First Century. A. Zihao. London, Routledge: 40–60.

  • Pigliucchi, M., Murren, C. J., et al. (2006). Phenotypic plasticity and evolution by genetic assimilation. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209(12), 2362–2367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum, L. A. (1978). The creation of a behavioral taxonomy. Observing behavior: Data collection and analysis methods. G. P. Sackett (pp. 15–24). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum, L.A. (1979). Monkeys in time and space. Social interaction analysis: Methodological issues. In M. Lamb, S. J. Suomi and G. Stephenson (ed.), (pp. 269–290). Madison, The University of Wisconsin Press.

  • Rosenblum, L. A., & Coe, C. L. (Eds.). (1985). The handbook of squirrel monkey research. Plenum Press: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum, L. A., & Paully, P. W., Jr. (1980). The social milieu of the developing monkey: studies of the development of social perception. Reproduction Nutrition Development, 20(3B), 827–841.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ruppert, E. E., Fox, R. S., et al. (2004). Invertebrate zoology: A functional evolutionary approach. Thomson: Belmont.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., et al. (2009). Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area. Science, 326(5951), 445–449. Words, grammar, and phonology are linguistically distinct, yet their neural.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sang-Hun, C. (2010). Rule of thumbs: Koreans reign in texting world. New York Times. New York, New York Times.

  • Swan, L. S., & Goldberg, L. J. (2010a). Biosymbols: symbols in life and mind. Biosemiotics, 3(1), 13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swan, L. S., & Goldberg, L. J. (2010b). How is meaning grounded in the organism? Biosemiotics, 3(2), 131–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Louis J. Goldberg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goldberg, L.J., Rosenblum, L.A. On the Genetic and Epigenetic Bases of Primate Signal Processing. Biosemiotics 6, 161–176 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-011-9135-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-011-9135-z

Keywords

Navigation