Skip to main content
Log in

A critique of information processing theories of consciousness

  • General Articles
  • Published:
Minds and Machines Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Information processing theories in psychology give rise to “executive” theories of consciousness. Roughly speaking, these theories maintain that consciousness is a centralized processor that we use when processing novel or complex stimuli. The computational assumptions driving the executive theories are closely tied to the computer metaphor. However, those who take the metaphor serious — as I believe psychologists who advocate the executive theories do — end up accepting too particular a notion of a computing device. In this essay, I examine the arguments from theoretical computational considerations that cognitive psychologists use to support their general approach in order to show that they make unwarranted assumptions about the processing attributes of consciousness. I then go on to examine the assumptions behind executive theories which grow out of the computer metaphor of cognitive psychology and conclude that we may not be the sort of computational machine cognitive psychology assumes and that cognitive psychology's approach in itself does not buy us anything in developing theories of consciousness. Hence, the state space in which we may locate consciousness is vast, even within an information processing framework.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, D. (1988), “What Concept Consciousness?”, in A. Marcel and E. Bisiach, eds.,Consciousness in Contemporary Science, Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 159–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. and G. Bower (1973),Human Associative Memory, Washington, D.C.: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C. and R. M. Shiffrin (1968), ‘Human Memory: A Proposed System and Its Control Processes’, in K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence eds.,The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, Volume 2. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1985), ‘Conscious Contents Provide the Nervous System with Coherent, Global Information’, in G. E. Schwartz and D. Shapiro, eds.,Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory, Volume 3. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. and Newman, G. (1988), ‘Convergent Cognitive and Neurophysiological Evidence for a Global Workspace Theory of Conscious Experience’, ms.

  • Baars, B. (1989),A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. and Logie, R. H. (forthcoming). ‘Auditory Imagery and Working Memory’, in D. Reisberg (ed.),Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Bergson, H. (1913/1962),Matter and Memory. London: G. Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. (1958),Perception and Communication. London: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherniak, C. (1986),Minimal Rationality, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, A. (1936), ‘An Unsolvable Problem of Elementary Number Theory’,American Journal of Mathematics 58, pp. 354–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. M. (1990), ‘Peer Commentary’,Social Epistemology 4, pp. 162–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S. (1986),Neurophilosophy: Toward a Unified Science of Mind-Brain, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S. (1987), ‘Replies to Comments’,Inquiry 29, pp. 241–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. and E. Loftus (1975), ‘A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing,Psychological Review 82, pp. 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1982), ‘The Demise of Short Term Memory’,Acta Psychologica 50, pp. 291–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1978), ‘Toward a Cognitive Theory of Consciousness’, in C. W. Savage, ed.,Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, Volume 9: Perception and Cognitions, Issues in the Foundation of Psychology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 201–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Everatt, J. and Underwood, G. (1992) “Parafoveal Guidance and Priming Effects During Reading: A Special Case of the Mind Being Ahead of the Eyes;Consciousness and Cognition 1, pp. 186–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1983),The Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P. (1990), ‘The Function of Plasticity’, ms.

  • Hatfield, G. (1988), ‘Neuro-Philosophy Meets Psychology: Reduction, Autonomy, and Physiological Constraints’,Cognitive Neuropsychology 5, pp. 723–746.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. (1949),The Organization of Behavior, New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1987),Consciousness and the Computational Mind, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890),The Principles of Psychology, New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. (1983),Mental Models, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. (1988), ‘A Computational Analysis of Consciousness’, in A. J. Marcel and E. Bisiach, eds.,Consciousness in Contemporary Science. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 357–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, E. R. and J. H. Schwartz (1985),Principles of Neural Science, Second Edition, New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1988), ‘The Child is a Theoretician, Not an Inductivist’,Mind and Language 3, pp. 183–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. (1973),Attention and Human Performance, Pacific Palisades, California: Goodyear.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1973a), ‘Attention and the Measurement of Perceptual Learning’,Memory and Cognition 1, pp. 268–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1973b), ‘Identification of the Time to Switch Attention: A Test of a Serial and Parallel Model of Attention’, in S. Kornblum, ed.,Attention and Performance IV. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1975), ‘Acquisition of Automatic Processing in Perceptual and Associative Learning,’ in P. M. A. Rabbitt and S. Dornic (eds.),Attention and Performance 10. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. and S. Samuels (1974), ‘Toward a Theory of Automatic Information Processing in Reading’,Cognitive Psychology 6, pp. 293–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1987),Foundations of Cognitive Grammar Standford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lycan, W. (1987),Consciousness, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975), ‘Consciousness: Respectable, Useful, and Probably Necessary’, in R. Solso, ed.,Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum, pp. 229–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1984),Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress, New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1985),Cognitive Psychology: An Essay in Cognitive Sciencel. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1988), ‘Problems and Direction in the Study of Consciousness’, in M.J. Horowitz, ed.,Psychodynamics and Cognition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 21–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1989), ‘Toward a Cognitive Theory of Consciousness’, ms.

  • Mandler, G. and Shebo, B. J. (1982), ‘Subitizing: An Analysis of Its Component Parts,’Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111, pp. 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G., E. Galanter, and K. Pribram (1960),Plans and the Structure of Behavior, New York: Henry Holt and Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967),Cognitive Psychology, New York: Appleton-Century-Croft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. and H. A. Simon (1972),Human Problem Solving, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. and H. A. Simon (1976), ‘Computer Science as Empirical Inquiry: Symbols and Search’,Communications of the ACM 19, pp. 113–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., J. C. Shaw, and H. A. Simon (1958), ‘Elements of a Theory of Human Problem Solving’,Psychological Review 65, 151–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. and T. Shallice (1988), ‘Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior’, in R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, and D. Shapiro, eds.,Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory, Volume 4. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K. (1988), ‘On Changing One's Mind: A Possible Function of Consciousness’, in A. Marcel and E. Bisiach, eds.,Consciousness in Contemporary Science. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 369–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. and C. Snyder (1975a), ‘Attention and Cognitive Control’, in R. Solso, ed.,Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. and C. Snyder (1975b), ‘Facilitation and Inhibition in the Processing of Signals’, in P. Rabbit and S. Dornic, eds.,Attention and Performance V. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. and R. Klein (1973), ‘On the Functions of Consciousness’, In S. Kornblum, ed.,Attention and Performance IV. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Post, E. L. (1936), ‘Finite Combinatory Processes - Formulation I’,Journal of Symbolic Logic 1, 103–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribot, T. (1890/1938),Psychologie de l'Attention. Paris: 7. Alcan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D., R. McClelland, and the PDP Research Group (1986),Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. and R. M. Shiffrin (1977), ‘Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and Attention’,Psychological Review 84, pp. 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1988), ‘Information-Processing Models of Consciousness: Possibilities and Problems’, in A. J. Marcel and E. Bisiach, eds.,Consciousness in Contemporary Science. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 305–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, G. (1979),The Synaptic Organization of the Brain, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. (1975) ‘The Locus and Role of Attention in Memory’, in P. Rabbitt and S. Dornic, eds.,Attention and Performance V. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. and W. Geisler (1973), ‘Visual Recognition in a Theory of Information Processing’, in R. Solso, ed.,Contemporary Issues in Cognitive Psychology: The Loyola Symposium. Washington, D.C.: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M. and W. Schneider (1977), ‘Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: II. Perceptual Learning, Automatic Attending, and General Theory’, Psychological Review 84, pp. 127–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E., W. Hirst, and U. Neisser (1976), ‘Skills of Undivided Attention’,Cognition 4, pp. 215–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stich, S. P. (1990),The Fragmentation of Reason: Preface to a Pragmatic Theory of Cognitive Evaluation, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A. M. (1936), ‘On Computable Numbers, With an Application to the Entscheidungsproblem,’Proceedings of the London Mathematics Society (Series 2), 42, 230–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. (1974), ‘Constructive Theory, Perceptual Systems, and Tacit Knowledge’, in W. Weld and D. Palermo, eds.,Cognition and Symbolic Processes. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, C. (1988), ‘The Control Operations of Consciousness’, in A. Marcel and E. Bisiach, eds.,Consciousness in Contemporary Science. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 334–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gulick, R. (1988), ‘A Functionalist Plea for Self-Consciousness’,Philosophical Review 48, pp. 149–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waugh, N. and D. Norman (1965), ‘Primary Memory’,Psychological Review 72, pp. 89–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegman, C. (1985),Psychoanalysis and Cognitive Psychology, New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hardcastle, V.G. A critique of information processing theories of consciousness. Mind Mach 5, 89–107 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00974191

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00974191

Key words

Navigation