Skip to main content
Log in

How Conservative Are Evolutionary Anthropologists?

A Survey of Political Attitudes

Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The application of evolutionary theory to human behavior has elicited a variety of critiques, some of which charge that this approach expresses or encourages conservative or reactionary political agendas. In a survey of graduate students in psychology, Tybur, Miller, and Gangestad (Human Nature, 18, 313–328, 2007) found that the political attitudes of those who use an evolutionary approach did not differ from those of other psychology grad students. Here, we present results from a directed online survey of a broad sample of graduate students in anthropology that assays political views. We found that evolutionary anthropology graduate students were very liberal in their political beliefs, overwhelmingly voted for a liberal U.S. presidential candidate in the 2008 election, and identified with liberal political parties; in this, they were almost indistinguishable from non-evolutionary anthropology students. Our results contradict the view that evolutionary anthropologists hold conservative or reactionary political views. We discuss some possible reasons for the persistence of this view in terms of the sociology of science.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In response to Joseph, Winterhalder (2002:14) wrote “In as much as there is no explicit discussion in Joseph’s essay of apologism, capitalism or the world-view of biology, it is difficult to read this statement except as a kind of code, one that calls up ideological disapproval based on undocumented allegations about the politics of a field and those who practice it. It accuses, but offers no content to which there can be a response.”

  2. We eliminated one item from the Tybur et al. (2007) study, “Religion is an important part of my life” (which was a “conservative” item categorized in the individual rights factor) because we felt that this item does not assess a person’s political views (i.e., liberals are often religious too). Further, Tybur and colleagues removed this item from one of their analyses since it is likely that evolutionary folks (independent of political attitudes) are comparatively less religious since evolutionary accounts of human origins are antagonistic with most religious accounts. We also made slight modifications to the wording of items 6, 7, and 13 for clarity.

  3. For this analysis we used one-sample t-test to compare item mean scores with the midpoint (0) of the scale. Since this midpoint represents moderate political beliefs, this analysis is rather generous to the EAPC hypothesis. That is, finding no statistically significant differences between the responses of EAs and the midpoint would tell us they are indistinguishable from moderates, rather than being truly conservative or right-wing.

References

  • Allen, E., et al. (1975). Against sociobiology. New York Review of Books, 22(182), 184–186 (November 13).

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H. (Ed.). (2006). Missing the revolution: Darwinism for social scientists. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, G. W., & Silverberg, J. (Eds.). (1980). Sociobiology: Beyond nature/nurture? AAAS Selected Symposium 35. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Turke, P. (Eds.). (1988). Human reproductive behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. A., & Irons, W. G. (Eds.). (1979). Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: An anthropological perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L., Chagnon, N. A., & Irons, W. G. (Eds.). (2000). Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological perspective. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dagg, A. I. (2005). “Love of shopping” is not a gene: Problems with Darwinian psychology. Montreal: Black Rose Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, P., & Feldman, M. (2003). Genes and cultures: what creates our behavioral phenome? Current Anthropology, 44, 87–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Steps toward the evolutionary psychology of a culture-dependent species. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), Innateness and the structure of the mind, Vol. II (pp. 91–117). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Frederick, D. A., et al. (2009). Analyzing evolutionary social science and its popularizations - a review of “the caveman mystique: Pop-Darwinism and the debates over sex, violence, and science”. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 301–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1980). Sociosexology. New York Review of Books, 26(21–22), 3–4. January 24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (2001). Life among the anthros. New York Review of Books, 48(2), 18–22. February 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, E. H. (2005). Controversial issues in evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 145–176). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. J. (1989). Primate visions: Gender, race, and nature in the world of modern science. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. B. (1997). Raising Darwin’s consciousness: female sexuality and the prehominid origins of patriarchy. Human Nature, 8, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. G. (1979). Natural selection, adaptation, and human social behavior. In N. A. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior (pp. 4–39). North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, S. (2000). Anthropological evolutionary ecology: a critique. Journal of Ecological Anthropology, 4, 6–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, C. (1991). Blood relations: Menstruation and the origins of culture. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2002). Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Levins, R., & Lewontin, R. (1998). Foreword. In A. H. Goodman, & T. L. Leatherman (Eds.), Building a new biocultural synthesis: Political-economic perspectives on human biology (pp. xi–xv). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

  • Mann, C. C. (2001). Scientific community: anthropological warfare. Science, 291, 416–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, J. (1999). Review of Demonic males by R. Wrangham & D. Peterson. Human Biology, 71, 143–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, J. (2000). Isn’t Aristotle dead? Discussion paper for session on “A critique of psychological Darwinism.” American Anthropological Association annual meeting, November, San Francisco. Retrieved from http://personal.uncc.edu/jmarks/interests/talks.html.

  • Masters, R. D. (1982). Is sociobiology reactionary? The political implications of inclusive-fitness theory. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 57, 275–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKinnon, S. (2005). Neo-liberal genetics: The myths and moral tales of evolutionary psychology. Chicago: Prickly Paradigm Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavelka, M. M. (2002). Resistance to the cross-species perspective in anthropology. In A. Fuentes & L. D. Wolfe (Eds.), Primates face to face: Conservation implications of human-nonhuman primate interconnections (pp. 25–44). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peregrine, P. N., Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (2003). Cross-cultural evaluation of predicted association between race and behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(5), 357–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, H., & Rose, S. (2000). Introduction. In H. Rose & S. Rose (Eds.), Alas poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary psychology (pp. 1–13). London: Harmony Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. D. (1976). The use and abuse of biology: An anthropological critique of sociobiology. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrale, U. (2000). Defenders of the truth: The battle for science in the sociology debate and beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. (1989). The limitations of medical ecology: the concept of adaptation in the context of social stratification and social transformation. Medical Anthropology, 10, 223–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. (1996). Farewell to adaptationism: unnatural selection and the politics of biology. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, n.s., 10(4), 496–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2011). Endless forms: human behavioral diversity and evolved universals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 366, 325–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., & Winterhalder, B. (Eds.). (1992). Evolutionary ecology and human behavior. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Hill, K. (2001). Controversies in the evolutionary social sciences: a guide for the perplexed. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 16, 128–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundation of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, T. (2005). Anthropological responsibilities, scientific ethics, and the ideology of “science”: What do we owe the Yanomami? In R. Borofsky (Ed.), Yanomami: The fierce controversy and what we can learn from it (pp. 198–209). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tybur, J. M., Miller, G. F., & Gangestad, S. G. (2007). Testing the controversy: an empirical examination of adaptationists’ political attitudes. Human Nature, 18, 313–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, S. L. (1978). Animal behavior and social anthropology. In M. Gregory, A. Silvers, & D. Sutch (Eds.), Sociobiology and human nature (pp. 53–74). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (2002). Behavioral and other human ecologies: critique, response and progress through criticism. Journal of Ecological Anthropology, 6, 4–23.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank William Irons for suggesting that we undertake this research. Wesley Allen-Arave, Bret Beheim, Lee Cronk, Edward Hagen, Raymond Hames, John R. Hibbing, Kim Hill, Jeremy Koster, Daniel Nettle, Benjamin Purzycki, Rob Quinlan, Joshua Tybur, and Bruce Winterhalder provided helpful comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. Finally, we thank Alyssa Fitzpatrick Harlow, Samuel Kim, David Armo, and Alexandra Futran for their research assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Henry F. Lyle III.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lyle, H.F., Smith, E.A. How Conservative Are Evolutionary Anthropologists?. Hum Nat 23, 306–322 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9150-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9150-z

Keywords

Navigation