Skip to main content
Log in

The Social Origins of Folk Epistemology

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Because reasoning allows us to justify our beliefs and evaluate these justifications it is central to folk epistemology. Following Sperber, and contrary to classical views, it will be argued that reasoning evolved not to complement individual cognition but as an argumentative device. This hypothesis is more consistent with the prevalence of the confirmation and disconfirmation biases. It will be suggested that these biases render the individual use of reasoning hazardous, but that when reasoning is used in its natural, argumentative, context they can represent a smart way to divide labor without loosing epistemic value.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. What is called here, following Sperber, intuitive mechanisms is also known as system 1 processes, or heuristic system. I prefer the term intuitive because: (i) it is more explicit than ‘system 1’, (ii) reasoning also has a very heuristic nature (in fact more so than most intuitive processes) and, (iii) it corresponds quite well with the naïve notion of intuition—something we feel is right without knowing the reasons.

  2. By specialized mechanism I mean ‘module’, but not so much as the word is often used in the cognitive sciences (following Fodor), but in the more general sense of a mechanism whose inner workings are mostly independent from other mechanisms.

  3. That is, provided that the other participants propose other non normative answers. The case in which one of the participants has the normatively correct answer and the good explanation for it hasn’t been tested, but given the results mentioned above and the fact that it is quite easy to demonstrate the correct answer, she should win the argument.

  4. Again, this is assuming that no participant is aware of the normative answer and the normative explanation. When this is the case, reasoning decreases the sunk costs fallacy (Simonson and Nye 1992).

  5. The benefits of information pooling have been well demonstrated in other animals, who seem to be using a form of the Condorcet Jury Theorem (Conradt and Roper 2003, 2005; List 2004). However, debate has features that render it quite different from other forms of opinion aggregation (such as voting—as in the CJT—or bargaining). There is no space here to discuss these issues further, but see Elster (1998) for an opinion that would be closed to the one that would be defended here.

References

  • Arkes, H.R., and P. Ayton. 1999. The sunk cost and Concorde effects: Are humans less rational than lower animals. Psychological Bulletin 125(5): 591–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonner, B.L., M.R. Baumann, and R.S. Dalal. 2002. The effects of member expertise on group decision making and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 88: 719–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M.M., T.S. Braver, D.M. Barch, C.S. Carter, and J.D. Cohen. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3): 624–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briley, D.A., M.W. Morris, and I. Simonson. 2000. Reasons as carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer Research 27(2): 157–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D.L., and R.M. Seyfarth. 1990. How monkeys see the world. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conradt, L., and T.J. Roper. 2003. Group decision-making in animals. Nature 421(6919): 155–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conradt, L., and T.J. Roper. 2005. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 20(8): 449–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, M., and R.M.J. Byrne. 2005. When falsification is the only path to truth. Paper presented at the Twenty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Stresa, Italy.

  • Davis, J.H. 1973. Group decisions and social interactions: A theory of social decision schemes. Psychological Review 80: 97–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B.M., J.J. Lindsay, B.E. Malone, L. Muhlenbruck, K. Charlton, and H. Cooper. 2003. Cues to deception. Psychol Bull 129(1): 74–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dessalles, J.-L. 2007. Why we talk: The evolutionary origins of language. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. 2004. Think different: the merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87(5): 586–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., M.W. Bos, L.F. Nordgren, and R.B. van Baaren. 2006. On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311(5763): 1005–1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ditto, P.H., and D.F. Lopez. 1992. Motivated skepticism: use of differential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions. 63(4): 568–584.

  • Edwards, K., and E.E. Smith. 1996. A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, L.C., L.R. Santos, and P. Bloom. 2007. The origins of cognitive dissonance: Evidence from children and monkeys. Psychological Science 18(11): 978–983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. 1998. Introduction. In Deliberative democracy, ed. J. Elster. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T. 2006. The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 13(3): 378–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J.S.B.T., and D.E. Over. 1996. Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S.T., A.J.C. Cuddy, and P. Glick. 2007. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(2): 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frantz, C.M., and R. Janoff-Bulman. 2000. Considering both sides: The limits of perspective taking. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 22: 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S. 2005. On the ball: Cognitive reflection and decision-making. Journal of Economic Perspectives.

  • Glachan, M., and P. Light. 1982. Peer interaction and learning: Can two wrongs make a right? In Social cognition: Studies in the development of understanding, ed. G. Butterworth and P. Light, 238–262. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouzoules, H., S. Gouzoules, and K. Miller. 1996. Skeptical responding in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). International Journal of Primatology 17: 549–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, J.B., and G.M. Levine. 1999. Effects of reasons analysis on the accuracy of predicting basketball games. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 29(3): 517–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, K.R. 1996. Human judgment and social policy: Irreducible uncertainty, inevitable error, unavoidable injustice. USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, R., and T. Kameda. 2005. The robust beauty of majority rules in group decisions. Psychological Review 112(2): 494–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., P. Slovic, and A. Tversky. 1982. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge University Press.

  • Koenig, M.A., and P.L. Harris. 2005. The role of social cognition in early trust. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(10): 457–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koopowitz, H., and L. Keenan. 1982. The primitive brains of platyhelminthes. Trends in Neurosciences 5(3): 77–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., S. Lichtenstein, and B. Fischhoff. 1980. Reasons for confidence. Journal of Experimenfal Psychology: Human Learning and Memory and Cognition 6: 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J.R., and R. Dawkins. 1984. Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation? In Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 2èmeth ed, ed. J.R. Krebs and N.B. Davies, 390–402. Oxford: Basil Blackwell Scientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J.L. 2003. Return of the ego-self-referent information as a filter for social prediction: Comment on Karniol (2003). Psychological Review 110: 585–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A.J., S. Cronen, A.L. Chasteen, and B. Lickel. 1996. Private vs public expressions of racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 32(5): 437–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P.R., and A.L. Ellis. 1986. Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 22: 177–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P.R., N.L. Kerr, J.H. Davis, H.M. Halff, and K.A. Marciniak. 1975. Group size, member ability, and social decision schemes on an intellective task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33: 80–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P.R., B.L. Bonner, and A.G. Miner. 2002. Groups perform better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers problems. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 88: 605–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J.S., and P.E. Tetlock. 1999. Accounting for the effects of accountability. Psychological Bulletin 125: 255–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, C. 2004. Democracy in animal groups: A political science perspective. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19(4): 168–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C.G., L. Ross, and M.R. Lepper. 1979. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37(11): 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C.G., M.R. Lepper, and E. Preston. 1984. Considering the opposite: A corrective strategy for social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47: 1231–1243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., and D.V. Budescu. 2007. Collective induction without cooperation? Learning and knowledge transfer in cooperative groups and competitive auctions. Journal of personality and social psychology 92(5): 854–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M.J., and B.G. DeMonbreun. 1977. Psychology of the scientist: An analysis of problem-solving bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research 1(3): 229–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, L.T., J. Trommershäuser, and M.S. Landy. 2007. Questions without words: A comparison between decision making under risk and movement planning under risk. In Integrated models of cognitive systems, ed. W.D. Gray, 297–313. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. submitted. Looking for arguments.

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2009. Intuitive and reflective inferential mechanisms. In In two minds, ed. J.S.B.T. Evans and K. Frankish. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. submitted. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory.

  • Michaelsen, L.K., W.E. Watson, and R.H. Black. 1989. A realistic test of individual versus group consensus decision making. Journal of Applied Psychology 74(5): 834–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moshman, D., and M. Geil. 1998. Collaborative Reasoning: Evidence for Collective Rationality. Thinking and Reasoning 4(3): 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R.S. 1998. Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomena in many guises. Review of General Psychology 2: 175–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R.E., and D.T. Wegener. 1998. Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In The handbook of social psychology, vol. 1, ed. D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, and G. Lindzey, 323–390. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poletiek, F.H. 1996. Paradoxes of falsification. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A: 447–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, T.J. 2007. Forced moves or good tricks in design space? Landmarks in the evolution of neural mechanisms for action selection. Adaptive Behavior 15(1): 9–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, T.J., P. Redgrave, and K.N. Gurney. 1999. Layered control architectures in robots and vertebrates. Adaptive Behavior 7(1): 99–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redgrave, P., T.J. Prescott, and K.N. Gurney. 1999. The basal ganglia: A vertebrate solution to the selection problem? Neuroscience 89: 1009–1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., and G. Wright. 1999. The Delphi technique as a forecasting tool: Issues and analysis. International Journal of Forecasting 15(4): 353–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz-Hardt, S., F.C. Brodbeck, A. Mojzisch, R. Kerschreiter, and D. Frey. 2006. Group decision making in hidden profile situations: Dissent as a facilitator for decision quality. Journal of personality and social psychology 91(6): 1080–1093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B.B., Y. Neuman, and S. Biezuner. 2000. Two wrongs make a right...if they argue together! Cognition and Instruction 18: 461–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, E., I. Simonson, and A. Tversky. 1993. Reason-based choice. Cognition 49(1–2): 11–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I. 1989. Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects. The Journal of Consumer Research 16(2): 158–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I., and P. Nye. 1992. The effect of accountability on susceptibility to decision errors. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 51(3): 416–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S.A. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119(1): 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P. 1975. Choice between equally valued alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology (Human Perception and Performance) 1(3): 280–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P. 1993. Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Analysis 13(6): 675–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sniezek, J.A., and R.A. Henry. 1989. Accuracy and confidence in group judgment. Organizational behavior and human decision processes(Print) 43(1): 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spellman, B.A. 1993. Implicit learning of base rates. Psycoloquy 4: 61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 1997. Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language 12(1): 67–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2000. Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. D. Sperber, 117–137. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2001. An evolutionary perspective on testimony and argumentation. Philosophical Topics 29: 401–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., F. Clément, C. Heintz, O. Mascaro, H. Mercier, G. Origgi, and D. Wilson. in press. Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language.

  • Spinoza, B. 1677. Ethics.

  • Stanovich, K.E. 2004. The Robot’s rebellion. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., and R.F. West. 2007. Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Thinking and Reasoning 13(3): 225–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stasson, M.F., T. Kameda, C.D. Parks, S.K. Zimmerman, and J.H. Davis. 1991. Effects of assigned group consensus requirement on group problem solving and group members’ learning. Social Psychology Quarterly 54: 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tindale, R.S., and S. Sheffey. 2002. Shared information, cognitive load, and group memory. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 5(1): 5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toplak, M.E., and K.E. Stanovich. 2003. Associations between myside bias on an informal reasoning task and amount of post-secondary education. Applied Cognitive Psychology 17: 851–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., and D. Kahneman. 1983. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review 90(4): 293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C. 1966. Reasoning. In New horizons in psychology: I, ed. B.M. Foss, 106–137. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whyte, G. 1993. Escalating commitment in individual and group decision making: A prospect theory approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 54(3): 430–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., and J.W. Schooler. 1991. Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Thinking 60(2): 181–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., and S.J. LaFleur. 1995. Knowing what you’ll do: Effects of analyzing reasons on self-prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68(1): 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., D.J. Lisle, J.W. Schooler, S.D. Hodges, K.J. Klaaren, and S.J. LaFleur. 1993. Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19(3): 331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yaniv, I., and E. Kleinberger. 2000. Advice taking in decision making: Egocentric discounting and reputation formation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 83: 260–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., and A. Zahavi. 1997. The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Dan Sperber’s work and his support have been instrumental in the writing of this article. I also wish to thank two anonymous reviewers and an editor for their comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hugo Mercier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mercier, H. The Social Origins of Folk Epistemology. Rev.Phil.Psych. 1, 499–514 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0021-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0021-4

Keywords

Navigation