Skip to main content
Log in

Binocular Rivalry and the Cerebral Hemispheres With a Note on the Correlates and Constitution of Visual Consciousness

  • Published:
Brain and Mind

Abstract

In addressing thescientific study of consciousness, Crick and Koch state, “It is probable that at any moment some active neuronal processes in your head correlate with consciousness, while others do not: what is the difference between them?” (1998, p. 97). Evidence from electrophysiological and brain-imaging studies of binocular rivalry supports the premise of this statement and answers to some extent, the question posed. I discuss these recent developments and outline the rationale and experimental evidence for the interhemispheric switch hypothesis of perceptual rivalry. According to this model, the perceptual alternations of rivalry reflect hemispheric alternations, suggesting that visual consciousness of rivalling stimuli may be unihemispheric at any one time (Miller et al., 2000). However, in this paper, I suggest that interhemispheric switching could involve alternating unihemispheric attentional selection of neuronal processes for access to visual consciousness. On this view, visual consciousness during rivalry could be bihemispheric because the processes constitutive of attentional selection may be distinct from those constitutive of visual consciousness. This is a special case of the important distinction between the neuronalcorrelates and constitution of visual consciousness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aafjes, M., Hueting, J.E. and Visser, P., 1966: Individual and interindividual differences in binocular retinal rivalry in man, Psychophysiology 3, 18-22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alais, D. and Blake, R., 1998: Interactions between global motion and local binocular rivalry. Vis. Res. 38, 637-644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alais, D. and Blake, R., 1999: Grouping visual features during binocular rivalry. Vis. Res. 39, 4341-4353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alais, D., O'shea, R.P., Mesana-Alias, C. and Wilson, I.G., 2000: On binocular alternation, Perception 29, 1437-1445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albright, T.D., Jessell, T.M., Kandel, E.R. and Posner, M.I., 2000: Neural science: A century of progress and the mysteries that remain. Neuron 25, S1-S55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, T.J. and Purves, D., 1997: Similarities in normal and binocularly rivalrous viewing. Proc. National Academy of Sciences 94, 9905-9908.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aston-Jones, G., Rajkowski, J. and Cohen, J., 1999: Role of Locus Coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility. Biol. Psychiat. 46, 1309-1320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J., Campbell, F.W., Fiorentini, A. and Maffei, L., 1973: The dependence of monocular rivalry on spatial frequency. Perception 2, 127-133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A.P., Thomas, M.S.C. and Cleeremans, A., 2000: Consciousness: mapping the theoretical landscape. TICS 4, 372-382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B.J., 1997: Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception and working memory. Consciousness and Cognition 6, 363-371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baloh, R.W., Honrubia, V. and Konrad, H.R., 1976: Periodic alternating nystagmus. Brain 99, 11-26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bamford, O.S. and Eccles, R., 1982: The central reciprocal control of nasal vasomotor oscillations. Pflügers Archiv-Eur. J. Physiol. 394, 139-143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., Padmanabhan, S., Clark, V. P., Karni, A., Prinster, A., Braun, A., Lalwani, A., Rauschecker, J.P., Turner, R. and Neville, H., 1997: Sentence reading: A functional MRI study at 4 Tesla. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 664-686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., 1989: A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychol. Rev. 96, 145-167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N., 1996: How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? TINS 19, 456-459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogen, J., Berker, E., Van Lancker, D., Sudia, S., Lipstad, B., Sadun, A. and Weekes, N., 1998: Left hemicerebrectomy: Vision, olfaction and mentation 45 years later. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 24. Los Angeles, California, U.S.A., November 7-12, Society for Neuroscience, Washington, D.C., p. 173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G., 1933: The Physical Dimension of Consciousness. New York: Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borsellino, A., de Marco, A., Alazetta, A., Rinesi, S. and Bartolini, B., 1972: Reversal time distribution in the perception of visual ambiguous stimuli. Kybernetik 10, 139-144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottini, G., Sterzi, R., Paulesu, E., Vallar, G., Cappa, S.F., Erminio, F., Passingham, R. E., Frith, C.D. and Frackowiack, S.J., 1994: Identification of the central vestibular projections in man: A positron emission tomography activation study. Exp. Brain. Res. 99, 164-169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breese, B., 1899: On Inhibition. Psychol. Monographs 3, 1-65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, F.W., Gilinsky, A.S., Howell, E.R., Riggs, L.A. and Atkinson, J., 1973: The dependence of monocular rivalry on orientation. Perception 2, 123-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappa, S., Sterzi, R., Vallar, G. and Bisiach, E., 1987: Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia after vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 25, 775-782.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D.J., 1996: The Conscious Mind: In Search of a Fundamental Theory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D.J., 2000: What is a neural correlate of consciousness? In T. Metzinger (ed.), Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions, Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, pp. 17-39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chelazzi, L. and Corbetta, M., 2000: Cortical mechanisms of visuospatial attention in the primate brain. In M.S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 667-686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chelazzi, L., Miller, E.K., Duncan, J. and Desimone, R., 1993: A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363, 345-347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A. and Haynes, J-D., 1999: Correlating consciousness: A view from empirical science. Revue Internationale de Philosophie 209, 387-420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, B., Reisine, H., Yokota, J. I. and Raphan, T., 1992: The nucleus of the optic tract. Ann. New York. Acad. Sci. 656, 277-296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Shulman, G.L. and Petersen, S.E., 1993: A PET study of visuospatial attention. J. Neurosci. 13, 1202-1226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., 1994: The Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul. New York: Touchstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., 1995: Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature 375, 121-123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., 1998: Consciousness and Neuroscience. Cerebr. Cort. 8, 97-107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R. and Duncan, J., 1995: Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193-222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diaz-Caneja E., 1928: Sur l'alternance binoculaire. Annales D'Ocykustuqye 165, 721-731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, A.K., Fries, P., König, P., Brecht, M. and Singer, W., 1999: Temporal binding, binocular rivalry, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8, 128-151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, A.K., Roelfsema, P.R., Fries, P., Brecht, M. and Singer, W., 1997: Role of the temporal domain for response selection and perceptual binding. Cerebr. Cort. 7, 571-582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel Jr, J., 1993: Surgical Treatment of the Epilepsies. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enoksson, P., 1963: Binocular rivalry and monocular dominance studied with optokinetic nystagmus. Acta Ophthalmologica 41, 544-563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R. and Kanwisher, N., 1998: A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598-601.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahle, M., 1982: Binocular rivalry: Suppression depends on orientation and spatial frequency. Vision Res. 22, 787-800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, O., 1995: Consciousness and the natural method. Neuropsychologia 33, 1103-1115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R., 1991: Binocular rivalry. In D. Regan (ed.), Vision and Visual Dysfunction. Volume 9: Binocular Vision, London: Macmillan, pp. 93-110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. and Check, R., 1966: Forced-choice form recognition during binocular rivalry. Psychonomic Science 6, 471-472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. and Check, R., 1968: Detection of motion during binocular rivalry suppression. J. Exp. Psychol. 78, 388-395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. and Herrmann, J., 1967: Stochastic properties of binocular rivalry alternations. Percept. Psychophys. 2, 432-436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R., Todd, S. and Bettinger, L.A., 1975: Optokinetic nystagmus as an objective indicator of binocular rivalry. Vision Res. 15, 849-853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fries, P., Roelfsema, P.R., Engel, A.K., Konig, P. and Singer, W., 1997: Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proc. National Academy of Sciences 94, 12699-12704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C., Perry, R. and Lumer, E., 1999: The neural correlates of conscious experience: an experimental framework. TICS 3, 105-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, A.F., Mustari, M.J., Robinson, F.R. and Kaneko, C.R. S., 1992: Visual signals in the nucleus of the optic tract and their brain stem destinations. Ann. New York. Acad. Sci. 656, 266-276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M.S. (ed.), 2000: The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, C.G., Rodman, H.R., Gochin, P.M. and Colombo, M.W., 1993: Inferior temporal cortex as a pattern recognition device. In E. Baum (ed.), Computational Learning and Cognition: Proceedings of the Third NEC Research Symposium, Philadelphia: Siam, pp. 44-73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillery, R.W., Feig, S.L. and Lozsádi, D.A., 1998: Paying attention to the thalamic reticular nucleus. TINS 21, 28-32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heeger, D.J., 1999: Linking visual perception with human brain activity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 474-479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K.M. and Van Den Abell, T., 1980: Right hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 30, 327-330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helmholtz, H. von, 1910/ 1962: Treatise on Physiological Optics, Volume 3. Dover, New York [1962]. English translation by J.P.C. Southall for the Optical Society of America (1925) from the 3rd German edn. of Handbuch der physiologischen Optik, Voss, Hamburg, 1910; first published in 1867, Voss, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I.P. and Rogers, B.J., 1995: Binocular Vision and Stereopsis. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. and Guilford, J.P., 1933: Fluctuation of an ambiguous figure in dementia praecox and in manic-depressive patients. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 27, 443-452.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W., 1890: The Principles of Psychology. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H. and Jessel, T.M., 2000: Principles of Neural Science. New York: McGraw-Hill Health Professions Division.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., 2001: Neural events and perceptual awareness. Cognition 79, 89-113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. and Chun, M.M., 1997: The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302-4311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kastner, S., Pinsk, M.A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R. and Ungerleider, L.G.: Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 22, 751-761.

  • Keil, A., Elbert, T., Rockstroh, B. and Ray, W. J., 1998: Dynamical aspects of motor and perceptual processes in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Res. 33, 169-178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinomura, S., Larsson, J., Balázs, G. and Roland, P.E., 1996: Activation by attention of the human reticular formation and thalamic intralaminar nuclei. Nature 271, 512-515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., 1977: Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry. In E.A. Weinstein and R.P. Friedland (eds), Advances in Neurology, Volume 18, New York: Raven Press, pp. 41-49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S.M., Pascual-Leone, A., Felician, O., Camposano, S., Keenan, J.P., Thompson, W.L., Ganis, G., Sukel, K.E. and Alpert, N.M., 1999: The role of Area 17 in visual imagery: Convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284, 167-170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, I., Papathomas T.V., Yang, M. and Fehér Á., 1996: When the brain changes its mind: Interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Proc. National Academy of Sciences 93, 15508-15511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kulikowski, J.J., 1992: Binocular chromatic rivalry and single vision. Ophthalmol. Physiol. Opt. 12, 168-170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lack, L., 1978: Selective Attention and the Control of Binocular Rivalry. Mouton: The Hague.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. and Blake, R., 1999: Rival ideas about binocular rivalry. Vision Res. 39, 1447-1454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehky, S.R., 1988: An astable multivibrator model of binocular rivalry. Perception 17, 215-228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leopold, D.A. and Logothetis, N.K., 1996: Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature 379, 549-553.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeVay, S., Connolly, M., Houde, J. and Van Essen, D.C., 1985: The complete pattern of ocular dominance stripes in the striate cortex and visual field of the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 5, 486-501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W., 1965: On Binocular Rivalry. The Netherlands: Royal Van Gorcum, Assen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis N.K., 1998a: Single units and conscious vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 353, 1801-1818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis, N.K., 1998b: Object vision and visual awareness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 536-544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis, N.K., Leopold, D.A. and Sheinberg, D.L., 1996: What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? Nature 380: 621-624.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis, N.K. and Schall, J.D., 1989: Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 245, 761-763.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S.J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S.A. and Desimone, R., 1997: Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 24-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S.J., Hillyard, S.A., Mangun, G.R. and Gazzaniga, M.S., 1989: Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients. Nature 342, 543-545.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J. and Rees, G., 1998: Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science 280, 1930-1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahner, M. and Bunge, M., 1997: Foundations of Biophilosophy. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangun, G.R., Amishi, P. JHA, Hopfinger, J.B. and Handy, T.C., 2000: The temporal dynamics and functional architecture of attentional processes in human extrastriate cortex. In M.S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 701-710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marder, E., 1998: From biophysics to models of network function. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 25-45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marder, E. and Calabrese, R.L., 1996: Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. Physiol. Rev. 76, 687-717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mega, M.S. and Cummings, J.L., 1997: The Cingulate and Cingulate Syndromes. In M.R. Trimble and J.L. Cummings (eds), Contemporary Behavioral Neuology, Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann, pp. 189-214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (ed.), 2000: Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel, F., 1972: Sleep and waking in cats with various sagittal sections of the brain. In J. Carnecek and F. Podivinsky (eds), Cerebral Interhemispheric Relations, Bratislava: Vydavatelstvo Slovenskej Akademie Vied, pp. 83-97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S.M., 1999: The phenomenal phenotype: Evolution and ontology. In Proc. 3rd Annual Meeting of the Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness, Consciousness and Self: Neural, Cognitive and Philosophical Issues, London, Ontario, Canada, June 4-7, p. 30.

  • Miller, S.M., Liu, G.B., Ngo, T.T., Hooper, G., Riek, S., Carson, R.G. and Pettigrew, J.D., 2000: Interhemispheric switching mediates perceptual rivalry. Curr. Biol. 10, 383-92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukhametov, L.M., Lyamin, O.I. and Polyakova, I.G., 1985: Interhemispheric asynchrony of the sleep EEG in northern fur seals. Experientia 41, 1034-1035.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukhametov, L.M., Supin, A.Y. and Polyakova, I.G., 1977: Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins. Brain Res. 134, 581-584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munk, M.H.J., Roelfsema, P.R., Konig, P., Engel, A.K. and Singer, W., 1996: Role of reticular activation in the modulation of intracortical synchronization. Science 272, 271-274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, J., Miezin, F. and Allman, J., 1981: Binocular rivalry in macaque monkeys and humans: A comparative study in perception, Behav. Anal. Lett. 1, 149-156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. and Baars, B.J., 1993: A neural attentional model for access to consciousness: A global workspace perspective. Concepts in Neuroscience 4, 255-290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ngo, T.T., Miller, S.M., Liu, G.B. and Pettigrew, J.D., 2000: Binocular rivalry and perceptual coherence. Curr. Biol. 10, R134-R136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nobre, A.C., Sebestyen, G.N., Gitelman, D.R., Mesulam, M.M., Frackowiak, R.S.J. and Frith, C.D., 1997: Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120, 515-533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ooi, T.L. and He, Z.J., 1999: Binocular rivalry and visual awareness: The role of attention. Perception 28, 551-574.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'shea, R.P. and Corballis, P.M., 2000: Binocular rivalry in a split-brain observer. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Fort Lauderdale, Florida, April 30-May 5, ARVO, Inc., Bethesda, MD, U.S.A., IOVS, Vol. 41, p. S732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papathomas, T.V., Kovács, I., Fehér, A. and Julesz, B., 1999: Visual dilemmas: Competition between eyes and between percepts in binocular rivalry. In E. Lepore and Z. Pylyshyn (eds), What is cognitive science? Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, pp. 263-294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pardo, J.L., Fox, P.T. and Raichle, M.E., 1991: Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 349, 61-64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parvizi J. and Damasio, A., 2001: Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition 79, 135-159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, J.D., Collin, S.P. and Ott, M., 1999: Convergence of highly-specialized behaviour, eye movements and visual optics in the sandlance (Teleostei) and the chameleon (Reptilia). Curr. Biol. 9, 421-424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, J.D. and Miller, S.M., 1998: A 'sticky' interhemispheric switch in bipolar disorder? Proc. Royal Society of London B. Biol. Sci. 265, 2141-2148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Place, U.T., 1990: E. G. Boring and the mind-brain identity theory. The British Psychological Society, History and Philosophy of Psychology Newsletter 11, 20-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polonsky, A., Blake, R., Braun, J. and Heeger, D.J., 2000: Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nature Neurosci. 3, 1153-1159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., 1994: Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 7398-7403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. and DiGirolamo, G.J., 2000: Attention in cognitive neuroscience: An overview. In M.S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 623-631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. and Petersen, S.E., 1990: The attention system of the human brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V.S., 1994: Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes and Freudian psychology. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 37, 291-333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattenborg, N.C., Lima, S.L. and Amlaner, C.J., 1999: Facultative control of avian unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predation. Behav. Brain Res. 105, 163-172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattenborg, N.C., Amlaner, C.J. and Lima, S.L., 2000: Behavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24, 817-842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A., 2000: Prospects for a scientific research program on consciousness. In T. Metzinger (ed.), Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 57-75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, J.H. and Desimone, R., 1999: The role of neural mechanisms of attention in solving the binding problem. Neuron 24, 19-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I.H. and Marshall, J.C. (eds), 1993: Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, T.W., 1997: Arousal systems and attentional processes. Biol. Psychol. 45, 57-71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarter, M. and Bruno, J.P., 2000: Cortical cholinergic inputs mediating arousal, attentional processing and dreaming: Differential afferent regulation of the basal forebrain by telencephalic and brainstem afferents. Neuroscience 95, 933-952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, B.B., Guirguis, M., Kenwood, C. and Herman, C.P., 1998: Asymmetrical hemispheric activation and behavioural persistence: Effects of unilateral muscle contractions. Neuropsychology 12, 526-532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, T.H., Haglund, M.M., Lettich, E. and Ojemann, G.A., 2000: Asymmetry of neuronal activity during extracellular microelectrode recording from left and right human temporal lobe neocortex during rhyming and line-matching. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 803-812.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sengpiel, F., 2000: Visual perception: An alternative view of perceptual rivalry. Curr. Biol. 10, R482-R485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannahoff-Khalsa, D., 1991: Lateralized rhythms of the central and autonomic nervous systems. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 11, 225-251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannahoff-Khalsa, D., 1993: The ultradian rhythm of alternating cerebral hemispheric activity. Int. J. Neurosci. 70, 285-298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheinberg, D.L. and Logothetis, N.K., 1997: The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. Proc. National Academy of Sciences 94, 3408-3413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, J.J.C., 1959: Sensations and brain processes. Philos. Rev. 68, 141-156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, N., Khan, R.M., Saltman, A., Sullivan, E.V. and Gabrieli, J.D.E., 1999: The world smells different to each nostril. Nature 402, 35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan, R., Russell, D.P., Edelman, G.M. and Tononi, G., 1999: Increased Synchronization of neuromagnetic responses during conscious perception. J. Neurosci. 19, 5435-5448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strüber, D. and Stadler, M., 1999: Differences in top-down influences on the reversal rate of different categories of reversible figures. Perception 28, 1185-1196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sturm, W., de Simone, A., Krause, B.J., Specht, K., Hesselmann, V., Radermacher, I., Herzog, H., Tellman, L., Müller-Gärtner, H.-W. and Willmes, K., 1999: Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 37, 797-805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers, R.A., 1997: Peripheral control and lateralization of birdsong. J. Neurobiol. 33, 632-652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Vaughan, J.T. and Kanwisher N., 1998: Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron 21, 753-759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., Srinivasan, R., Russell, D.P. and Edelman, G.M., 1998: Investigating neural correlates of conscious perception by frequency-tagged neuromagnetic responses. Proc. National Academy of Sciences 95, 3198-3203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., 1962: Binocular rivalry and stereoscopic depth perception. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 14, 23-37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G., Bottini, G., Rusconi, M.L. and Sterzi, R., 1993: Exploring somatosensory hemineglect by vestibular stimulation. Brain 116, 71-86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitte, E., Derosier, C., Caritu, Y., Berthoz, A., Hasboun, D. and Soulie, D., 1996: Activation of the hippocampal formation by vestibular stimulation: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 523-526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade, N.J., 1998: Early studies of eye dominances. Laterality 3, 97-108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waespe, W., Cohen, B. and Raphan, T., 1985: Dynamic modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex by the nodulus and uvula. Science 228, 199-202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wales, R. and Fox, R., 1970: Increment detection thresholds during binocular rivalry suppression. Percept. Psychophys. 8, 90-94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, P., 1975: Stochastic properties of binocular rivalry alternations. Percept. Psychophys. 18, 467-473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, P., 1978: Binocular rivalry: Central or peripheral selective processes? Psychol. Bull. 85, 376-389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallman, J., Pettigrew, J.D. and Fritsches, K., 1995: Hemispheric switching of eye movements in sandlances. In Proc. 4th International Congress of Neuroethology, Nervous Systems and Behaviour, New York: Thieme Medical Publishers, p. 251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wei, M. and Sun, F., 1998: The alternation of optokinetic responses driven by moving stimuli in humans. Brain Res. 813, 406-410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werntz, D., Bickford, R.G., Bloom F.E. and Shannahoff-Khalsa, D.S., 1983: Alternating cerebral hemispheric activation and the lateralization of autonomic nervous function. Human Neurobiol. 2, 39-43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittle, P., Bloor, D.C. and Pocock, S., 1968: Some experiments on figural effects in binocular rivalry. Percept. Psychophys. 4, 183-188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyss, J.M. and Sripanidkulchai, K., 1984: The topography of the mesencephalic and pontine projections from the cingulate cortex of the rat. Brain Res. 293, 1-15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaidel, E., Clarke, J.M. and Suyenobu, B., 1990: Hemispheric independence: A paradigm case for cognitive neuroscience. In A.B. Scheibel and A. Wechsler (eds), Neurobiology of Higher Cognitive Function, New York: Guilford Press, pp. 297-355.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Miller, S.M. Binocular Rivalry and the Cerebral Hemispheres With a Note on the Correlates and Constitution of Visual Consciousness. Brain and Mind 2, 119–149 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017981619014

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017981619014

Navigation