Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 June 2013
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

“Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: how bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors

  • 1Atelier de la Conception de l'Espace, Institute of Architecture and the City, School of Architecture, Civil and Environmental Engineering, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 2Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Brain Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 3Immersive Interaction Group, Faculté d'Informatique et Communications, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 4Center of Neuroprosthetics, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Neurology, Geneva University Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland

Over the centuries architectural theory evolved several notions of embodiment, proposing in the nineteenth and twentieth century that architectonic experience is related to physiological responses of the observer. Recent advances in the cognitive neuroscience of embodiment (or bodily self-consciousness) enable empirical studies of architectonic embodiment. Here, we investigated how architecture modulates bodily self-consciousness by adapting a video-based virtual reality (VR) setup previously used to investigate visuo-tactile mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. While standing in two different interiors, participants were filmed from behind and watched their own virtual body online on a head-mounted display (HMD). Visuo-tactile strokes were applied in synchronous or asynchronous mode to the participants and their virtual body. Two interiors were simulated in the laboratory by placing the sidewalls either far or near from the participants, generating a large and narrow room. We tested if bodily self-consciousness was differently modulated when participants were exposed to both rooms and whether these changes depend on visuo-tactile stimulation. We measured illusory touch, self-identification, and performed length estimations. Our data show that synchronous stroking of the physical and the virtual body induces illusory touch and self-identification with the virtual body, independent of room-size. Moreover, in the narrow room we observed weak feelings of illusory touch with the sidewalls and of approaching walls. These subjective changes were complemented by a stroking-dependent modulation of length estimation only in the narrow room with participants judging the room-size more accurately during conditions of illusory self-identification. We discuss our findings and previous notions of architectonic embodiment in the context of the cognitive neuroscience of bodily self-consciousness and propose an empirical framework grounded in architecture, cognitive neuroscience, and VR.

Introduction

Inspired by Vitruvian theory, architects evolved several notions of embodiment (Vitruvius, 1st century BC). The classic ideal of architectonic embodiment, characterized as the bodily experience of a modular interior (e.g., as observed in the Pantheon in Rome), was further developed by Renaissance architects (Alberti, 1988). They proposed to enhance architectonic embodiment by combining modularity with experienced aspects of verticality (e.g., as known from the gothic domes) into a coherent architectonic form (Argan, 1946). Architectonic cues were thereby used to establish a sequence of predefined view and vanishing points in space, guiding the observer's body and eye movements through linear perspective according to the most advantageous effects of the architectonic forms. The thereby evoked impression of an architectonic continuum, comprehensive of modularity and verticality from a phenomenological point of view, has since then been referred to as the ideal of the Renaissance (Burckhardt, 1860).

Under the influence of the empirical biological sciences the notion of architectonic embodiment has been revisited in the nineteenth century, highlighting physiological and psychological aspects (Semper, 1860; Vischer, 1872; Lotze, 1884). Along these lines art historians have argued that the reference to the human body, by means of massive tectonic structures (i.e., crafted vertical elements) or spatial modules (i.e., regularly shaped void interiors) may facilitate the observer's identification with the environment through mechanisms of corporeal resonance (Wölfflin, 1886; Schmarsow, 1893). Particularly, Heinrich Wölfflin associated the physiognomy of the human body to architectonic cues based on oculomotor, somatosensory, and vestibular responses. Although the multisensory nature of architectonic embodiment is prominently present in Wölfflin's and others' theories, the exact role of own-body signals in the experience and appreciation of architecture has to date never been studied empirically.

In cognitive neuroscience embodiment and bodily self-consciousness have been associated with the feeling of owning a body, i.e., body ownership or self-identification (Tsakiris et al., 2007; Salomon et al., 2012), and the feeling of being located at one specific position in space, i.e., self-location (Schwabe and Blanke, 2008; Blanke and Metzinger, 2009). Studies in cognitive neuroscience have shown that self-identification and self-location can be modulated experimentally through visuo-tactile conflicts in healthy participants using immersive virtual reality (VR) to induce the so-called Full-Body-Illusion (FBI; Ehrsson, 2007; Lenggenhager et al., 2007; Petkova and Ehrsson, 2008; Aspell et al., 2009; Slater et al., 2009).

In the mentioned studies, participants wearing a head-mounted display (HMD) were filmed from behind at a distance of two meters and the filmed scene was projected on their HMD (Lenggenhager et al., 2007; Aspell et al., 2009). While participants were stroked with a rod on their backs, they were seeing on the HMD their own videotaped back or virtual body being stroked in front of them either in real-time, i.e., synchronous stroking, or with addition of a short delay, i.e., asynchronous stroking. In the synchronous condition participants reported illusory touch, that is, the feeling of being touched where they saw the virtual body being touched, and the feeling of self-identifying with the virtual body, that is, they felt as if the virtual body was their body (illusory self-identification). In addition, a change in self-location was found, meaning that participants showed a drift in self-location toward the virtual body. Both effects of self-identification and self-location were smaller or abolished for asynchronous stroking and also when the participants were presented a human-sized box as control object (Lenggenhager et al., 2007). Other studies have reported illusory ownership and self-identification with non-bodily objects in peripersonal space induced by visuo-tactile stroking (Armel and Ramachandran, 2003; Hohwy and Paton, 2010). Yet, how self-identification is influenced by visuo-spatial properties of the (architectonic) environment and vice versa has not been investigated previously.

Following principles of ecological psychology and embodied perception in experimental psychology, recent research has further revealed that own-body representations may influence how an observer perceives an environment (Gibson, 1979; Neisser, 1988). It has thus been demonstrated that perceived lengths in a visually presented environment might be influenced by the seen scale of one's own body within such an environment (Witt et al., 2005; Linkenauger et al., 2010). Moreover, such altered own-body perceptions seem to be modulated by experimentally induced changes in body ownership (van der Hoort et al., 2011). With respect to embodied perception in immersive VR it has also been shown that having a virtual representation of one's body in an immersive virtual environment helps to improve distance estimation (Mohler et al., 2010).

Here, we performed an experiment on the effects of two distinct architectonic interiors on bodily self-consciousness. We inquired whether experimentally induced illusory touch and self-identification (in the above described experimental setup; Lenggenhager et al., 2007) depends on the size of the architectonic interior by presenting the virtual body in a narrow or a large room and by exposing participants to synchronous and asynchronous visuo-tactile stimulation. To modulate the effect of room-size we placed two sidewalls next to the participants either in extrapersonal space, i.e., beyond reach (large room-size condition), or in peripersonal space, i.e., the space surrounding the body (narrow room-size condition; Fogassi et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1997; Ladavas et al., 1998; Teneggi et al., 2013). Additionally, we measured whether our participants' capacity to perceive the architectonic space dimensions changed during the different experimental conditions, expecting an improved ability during the illusion conditions. To this purpose we asked our participants to judge the length of several lines that we presented in the architectonic space during the different experimental conditions.

Our results show that synchronous stroking of the participant's body and the seen virtual body induces illusory touch and self-identification with the virtual body—independent from room-size. Furthermore, we found that weak feelings of illusory touch with the sidewalls and feelings of room retraction were induced in the narrow room. These subjective changes were complemented by a stroking-dependent modulation of length estimation only in the narrow room with participants judging the room dimensions more accurately during conditions of illusory self-identification. We discuss these data with respect to cognitive neuroscience of embodiment and bodily self-consciousness, and relate our data to theories of architectonic embodiment and VR.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-four healthy, right-handed participants (5 females; 23.5 ± 2.8 years; mean age ± SD) participated in the study. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision and no history of neurological or psychiatric conditions. Participants were sampled from the EPFL population. They were remunerated with 20 Sfr. an hour.

Ethics Statement

All participants gave written informed consent to the experiment. The study protocol was approved by the local ethics research committee—La Commission d'Ethique de la Recherche Clinique de la Faculté de Biologie et de Medecine at the University of Lausanne, Switzerland and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Virtual Reality Setup

We used the video-based VR setup as previously described in Lenggenhager et al. (2007) and Aspell et al. (2009) and adapted it in such a way that the image seen by the participant would include a much larger proportion of space surrounding the virtual body (Figure 1). In the above-mentioned studies only the upper part of the participant's body was shown in a neutral environment and without any particular environmental cues. Also, the virtual body appeared to be closer to the participants' viewpoint, filling their visual field with the virtual representation of their own upper body. In our setup, however, the virtual body was entirely embedded in a virtual scene representing two distinct architectonic interiors, large and narrow, with the same room depth. The entire scene was filmed and dispatched in stereo, rendering the impression of a three-dimensional architectonic envelope that contained the fully embedded virtual body. For this purpose we used 2 webcams (Logitech C510, Apples, Switzerland) fixed on a tripod with 7 cm separation using a custom support to generate a stereo image, projected on a high definition Head-Mounted-Display (HMD, model Virtual Research Systems VR1280, Aptos, CA), rendering 2 images of 1280 × 1024 pixels at 60 Hz, with a diagonal field of view of 60°. The cameras were centered on the backside of either room, aligned at the same distance and directed toward the front wall.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The architectonic setup and the participant's view. The setup with the movable walls and backstroking for the large room (A) and the narrow room (B). The participant's impression of space viewed through the HMD for the large (C) and narrow room-sizes (D). Filming of the scene in stereo 3D with two webcams combined to top lighting allowed a realistic representation of depth.

In order to change the size of the room (factor room-size) in which both, the subject and the virtual body were standing, we used 4 custom-made panels (1.50 × 2.50 meters, length × height) mounted on wheels, aligned as one 6 meters long movable wall (Figures 1A,B). In combination with one corner of the experimentation room the movable walls formed either a large room (surface 6 × 3.50 meters, length × width) or a narrow room (surface 6 × 0.80 meters, length × width). Synchronous or asynchronous stroking (factor stroking) was applied to the back of the participants by the use of a wooden stick as described previously (Figures 1C,D) (Lenggenhager et al., 2007). For synchronous stroking the image captured from the webcams was directly relayed to the HMD. For asynchronous stroking a delay (800 ms) was inserted.

Length estimation was tested with previously captured images of the two interiors. The images showed the same setup from the identical visual angle, yet, including some black bars presented in different positions and orientations (Figure 2). A vertical black bar (50 cm) placed at the bottom center of the white front wall was used as the reference bar for the estimations. The bars involved in the length estimation consisted of: 5 horizontal bars placed on the bottom of the front wall (lengths of 40, 45, 50, 55, and 60 cm); 5 bars seen on the ground in perspective and centered with respect to the room-size (lengths 40, 50, 60, 70, and 80 cm); 5 bars seen on the ground along the right wall (lengths 40, 50, 60, 70, and 80 cm; Figure 2); and 5 bars seen on the ground along the left wall of lengths 40, 50, 60, 70, and 80 cm. For each of the four experimental conditions we presented a total of 20 bars in randomized order.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Stimuli for length estimation presented in perspective. The stimuli of the length estimation task (black bars) as presented on the HMD after each block of the FBI for the two interiors. The bars were presented in perspective along the sidewalls of the large (A) and narrow room (B).

Procedure

Participants were asked to wear the HMD for the whole duration of each experimental condition and to stand at the center of the room (large or narrow) facing the front wall at a distance of 4.5 meters from the webcams (Figure 1). Before carrying out the experimental conditions we performed a training session, allowing the study participants to familiarize with the setup. The vertical reference bar was presented to the participants, followed by one shorter and one longer horizontal bar. For each subject the cameras and the HMD were calibrated and adjusted ensuring a correct visual angle throughout the four conditions.

The four experimental conditions were run in separate blocks with randomization of factors: room-size (large/narrow) and stroking (synchronous/asynchronous) in a full 2 × 2 factorial design. Participants were filmed from behind by the webcams and watched, on the HMD, their body standing in front of them as a virtual body (Figures 1C,D). First, participants were exposed to the reference bar and then to visuo-tactile stroking for 2 min. Then participants were asked to perform a two-alternative forced choice task, responding whether they perceived the presented bars as longer or shorter than the reference bar (Figure 2). Responses were collected by means of a joystick (Logitech Cordless RumblePad 2). During the four experimental conditions the participants were exposed to white noise over the HMD headphones. ExpyVR (http://lnco.epfl.ch/expyvr) software was used to render the stimuli and record the responses. Size estimation was carried out immediately after the 2 min stroking period. At the end of each experimental condition, the HMD was removed and participants were requested to answer a 14 item written questionnaire and to rate each item on a Likert scale between −3 (not at all) and +3 (very much) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The questionnaire completed after each experimental block.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was performed with Matlab R2012a (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA) and Statistica 10 (Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK). Questionnaire scores were analyzed using repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) with the factors stroking (synchronous/asynchronous), room-size (large/narrow), and question (1–14; Table 1) as within-participant factors. For further post-hoc tests for specific significant interactions we performed t-tests. In order to avoid false positives we excluded any significant answer that was not confirmed if tested with an alternative and more critical non-parametric test (Wilcoxon test).

The analysis of performance in the length estimation task was done using Probit analysis (Craven, 1993; Howe and Purves, 2002, 2005). Probit analysis provides a method to determine the error distribution in the length estimation of bars of different lengths presented in a task in comparison to a reference bar. The experimental factors were introduced as possible indicators of the variations of estimation. Since Probit analysis is a particular type of generalized linear model, we normalized all the experimental factors to introduce the Beta values as indicators of the relative strength of the different factors. A factor was considered to be relevant, if it showed a significance level of p < 0.05. In addition, the factor was only included if it resulted in a significantly smaller residual error of the fit (χ2 comparison test, p < 0.05).

To the best of our knowledge Probit analysis has not been used previously in experiments addressing bodily self-consciousness. Therefore, as a complementary analysis on the same data we considered the subjective length estimations (of each particular bar length and position) in relation to the probability threshold of perceiving the bars as shorter than the reference bar. For example, a probability threshold of 0.5 corresponds to a bar length subjectively estimated as being equal to the length of the reference stimulus [for example see Kannape et al. (2010) for a similar measure]. We excluded those participants, whose subjective estimation thresholds were outside the range of estimation of the stimuli presented for any chosen threshold. A pilot study revealed the tendency to underestimate the length of the presented stimuli. Hence, in order to include a greater amount of participants, we favored a 0.75 probability threshold of answering shorter, instead of a 0.50 probability threshold of equal length (ruled out by the forced-choice task). This was analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni post-hoc tests.

Results

Questionnaire Responses

Questionnaire analysis revealed the following two-way interactions: question by room-size [Partial η2 = 0.165, F(13, 299) = 4.54, p < 0.001] and question by stroking [Partial η2 = 0.428, F(13, 299) = 17.22, p < 0.001; Table 1]. We also found the following main effects: room-size [Partial η2 = 0.190, F(1, 23) = 5.4069, p = 0.0292], stroking [Partial η2 = 0.555, F(1, 23) = 28.72, p < 0.001] and question [Partial η2 = 0.535, F(13, 299) = 26.47, p < 0.001]. No other effects were significant.

Further post-hoc testing of the question by stroking interaction revealed a significant variation with stroking for three questions (Figure 3A; Table 1). These were question 1 [… I was feeling the touch of the stick where I saw the virtual body being touched; t(1, 23) = 9.3134, p < 0.001], where the synchronous condition scored higher (mean = 2.52, s.e. = 0.18) than the asynchronous condition (mean = −1.10, s.e. = 0.42); question 2 [… I clearly felt that the stick touching the virtual body was causing the touch I was feeling; t(1, 23) = 5.6168, p < 0.001], where again the synchronous condition revealed a higher score (mean = 2.02, s.e. = 0.36) than the asynchronous condition (mean = −0.86, s.e. = 0.46) and question 3 [… I clearly felt that the virtual body was my body; t(1, 23) = 2.6602, p = 0.014], where the synchronous condition also showed higher scores (mean = 1.62, s.e. = 0.39) than the asynchronous (mean = 0.62, s.e. = 0.47). None of the control questions was significantly modulated by factor stroking.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Subjective responses. (A) Bodily self-consciousness. Results of the questionnaire responses are shown (mean and standard error) for the items that depended on stroking (questions 1 and 3). (B) Architectonic embodiment. Results of the questionnaire responses are shown (mean and standard error) for the items that depended on room-size (questions 7 and 12).

Further post-hoc testing with respect to the question by room-size interaction showed a significant variation for question 7 and 12 (Figure 3B; Table 1). Question 7 referred to illusory touch with the architectonic interior (… I felt as if the walls of the room were almost lightly touching me). Although ratings were low we found question 7 to be significantly stronger [t(1, 23) = 3.5973, p = 0.002] in the narrow (mean = −0.73, s.e. = 0.39) as compared to the large room condition (mean = −2.06, s.e. = 0.29). Ratings for question 12 were also low and inquired about room retraction (… I felt as if the walls were getting closer to myself). This was found to be significantly stronger [t(1, 23) = 3.1759, p = 0.030] in the narrow room (mean = −1.2, s.e. = 0.35) than in the large room (mean = −1.90, s.e. = 0.32). No further question was modulated by factor room-size.

Length Estimation

Several factors significantly contributed to the probability of perceiving the bars shorter than the reference bar (Figure 4A). As expected the main factor was the length of the presented bars (beta = −1.154, p < 0.001). The factor room-size (beta = −0.2785, p < 0.001) and the interaction between stroking and room-size were also significant (beta = 0.1017, p = 0.018). No other factors, such as the bar positions, affected the responses.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Bodily self-consciousness modulates length estimation. (A) The result of the Probit analysis shown for the four conditions across all bar lengths. The fit of the curves together with the confidence intervals suggest that the modulation in length estimation is specific for the narrow room and modulated by the stroking synchrony factor. (B) The effect of the stroking synchrony factor on the responses, independent of the presented bar length, is shown considering only responses in the narrow room. A Probit analysis shows a significant effect of the stroking synchrony factor, irrespective of the bar length. (C) The same Probit analysis considering only responses from the large room shows no significant effect of the stroking synchrony factor, (D) An analysis of variance of the subjective bar length corresponding to a 0.75 probability threshold shows an interaction between the stroking synchrony factor and room size. Bonferroni post-hoc tests show significant differences between synchronous and asynchronous stroking only in the narrow room. There are also differences between the large and the narrow room, but only for asynchronous stroking. *p < 0.05

In order to further analyze the stroking by room-size interaction we repeated the Probit analysis considering the large and narrow room-size conditions separately. This analysis revealed that the synchronous narrow room condition was the main driving factor for the interaction (beta = −0.1209, p = 0.049; Figure 4B). The same analysis for the large room condition was not significant when considered separately (beta = 0.0827, p = 0.170; Figure 4C), suggesting that stroking synchrony only affected length estimations when participants performed the estimations in the narrow room.

The same results were found in a complementary analysis using repeated measures ANOVA on the subjective bar length estimation thresholds. The subjective threshold of 0.75 probability of answering shorter included 19 of 24 participants (see the methods section for the exclusion criterion). The interaction room-size by stroking was significant [Partial η2 = 0.318, F(1, 18) = 8.389, p = 0.01]. Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between large and narrow room-size conditions for asynchronous stroking (p = 0.011). Stroking was also a significant factor, yet only in the narrow room-size condition (p = 0.043; see Figure 4D). Overall, ANOVA and Bonferroni post-hoc tests confirmed the interaction factor found in the Probit analysis for the subjective length at a 0.75 probability level.

The length estimations for the horizontal bars positioned along the front wall showed a different behavior. The length of the bars was revealed to be the main factor (p < 0.001). Stroking and the interaction stroking by room-size were not found to be significant (but revealed a non-significant trend; p = 0.08). No other significant factors were found.

The data on length estimation show that across all conditions participants estimated the bars presented in perspective as shorter than the target line. However, in the narrow room, length estimation significantly improved when participants self-identified with the virtual body (synchronous narrow condition), compared to when self-identification was not present (asynchronous narrow condition). This effect did not occur for the large room.

Discussion

Our data show that synchronous stroking of the participant's body and the seen virtual body induces illusory touch and self-identification with the virtual body. Self-identification and illusory touch were not differently modulated by the two different room-sizes. On the other hand, weak feelings of illusory touch with the sidewalls and the feeling of the approaching walls (room retraction) were induced by the narrow room-size. These subjective changes were complemented by a stroking-dependent modulation of length estimation only in the narrow room with participants judging the room dimensions more accurately during conditions of illusory self-identification.

Architectonic Experience and Visuo-Tactile Bimodality

The FBI is characterized by self-identification with the virtual body and a measured drift in self-location toward the virtual body. In our experimental setup a FBI was induced through visuo-tactile conflicts between felt touches applied to the participants' back and seen touches applied to the back of a virtual body. Participants were filmed from behind and their image was dispatched on their HMD, as previously tested by Lenggenhager et al. (2007) and Aspell et al. (2009). Our results confirm the outcome of the mentioned studies with respect to illusory touch and self-identification. In the questionnaires we did not find these aspects of bodily self-consciousness to be directly modulated by the two different room sizes.

Asking participants to judge their experiences of the interiors after the FBI, we found that they experienced very mild sensations of being touched by the sidewalls as well as the feeling that the sidewalls were drifting toward them (retraction). Both sensations were stroking-independent and differed for both room sizes, being stronger in the narrow room-size condition. This finding may suggest a mild effect of embodiment of the walls (touch) and of containment (experienced retraction of the sidewalls) induced by the room-size.

Previous studies between a participant's stroked physical hidden hand and seen strokes applied to a tabletop or non-human object induced feelings of illusory touch for the table by stroking (Armel and Ramachandran, 2003; Hohwy and Paton, 2010). Describing a touch illusion induced through visuo-tactile stroking of a white box and a hidden participant's hand, Hohwy and Paton (2010) found that the physical hand could be felt to extend toward the box. It was argued that the participants probably experienced the hand to be located inside the box and therefore the touch was felt as if “through the box.” In the questionnaire responses of our study, participants did not declare such mediating role of stroking with respect to the entire body, nor the whole interior. We argue that the present illusion of feeling touched by the sidewalls could only be induced visually through the architectonic elements located in peripersonal space, and without any dependence on visuo-tactile stroking as observed by others for a table or box (Armel and Ramachandran, 2003; Hohwy and Paton, 2010).

These visually induced touch sensations in conditions with closer walls seem to relate to previous findings about increased stimulus detection in peripersonal space (Rizzolatti et al., 1981). Several studies have since revealed visuo-tactile integration, especially when in close proximity to the body of the observer (Ladavas et al., 1998; Haggard et al., 2007; Sambo and Forster, 2009; Teneggi et al., 2013). We thus speculate that the narrow room, which was perceived within peripersonal space (10 cm or less lateral distance from participants' physical body), may be embodied visually and associated with mild feelings of touch and containment (room retraction). This effect was absent when the architectonic boundaries were outside peripersonal space, that is, in more distant extrapersonal space. Visuo-spatial perception in the large room might therefore be related to distinct perceptual and bodily mechanisms, such as visual perception linked to oculomotor processes (Witt and Proffitt, 2007). With respect to self-location in an interior space, this result suggests that bodily self-consciousness is extended toward those architectonic elements that are located in close proximity to the physical body.

Visuo-Spatial Estimation Depends on the Experience of Space

We introduced a behavioral measure that would allow testing the participants' ability to perceive the depth of the room, in function of the FBI. In our hypothesis we assumed that an increased ability of length estimation in three-dimensional space would correlate with the participants' experience of having their center of awareness inside the virtual room and body, i.e. through self-location and self-identification. Indeed, biases in the length estimation task were significantly modulated by the experimental conditions and associated with changes in self-identification and room-size experience. We also note that, consistent with previous work on length estimation, changes in length estimation affected all tested lengths in the same fashion (Craven, 1993; Howe and Purves, 2002, 2005).

The estimations were modulated in a stroking- and room-size-dependent fashion, as the interaction between stroking and room-size revealed. We expected that stroking would improve length estimations by increasing the sensation of spatial depth through multisensory perception of the virtual interior (associated with conditions of higher self-identification with the virtual body). Compared to the narrow room, in the large room length estimations were generally improved yet independently from stroking (i.e., synchronous vs. asynchronous stroking did not affect length estimations in the large room).

A possible interpretation of the difference between the large and the narrow interior may be given by the visual scaling of the virtual body inside the changed room proportions, as has already been argued before (Witt et al., 2005; Linkenauger et al., 2010). Such scaling-related visuo-spatial perception has also been shown to be related to self-identification with virtual space and body (van der Hoort et al., 2011). In addition, self-identification with the virtual body may have also induced a transposition of the center of perspective toward the virtual interior: in the narrow room this may have led participants to perceive the otherwise underestimated length more realistically, as it has been shown that self-identification with an avatar can reduce HMD-induced space-compression (Mohler et al., 2010). Since in the large room the FBI did not affect length estimations, we propose that architectonic elements close to the body increased the effect of spatial embodiment in combination to self-identification with the virtual body.

These findings are in line with investigations of embodied perception and notions of ecological psychology, stating that vision depends in a fundamental way on the experience of the body in space (Gibson, 1979; Neisser, 1988). Our results indicate that self-identification with the virtual body during the FBI may have mentally displaced the observer into the architectonic space when mediated through a visual stimulus in peripersonal space. This suggests that self-identification can be extended to architectonic elements close to the body in contrast to a human-sized object (box) shown in extra-personal space (Lenggenhager et al., 2007). These findings are compatible not only with a somatosensory impact of architecture through visual stimulation (as tested here in Question 7 and 12), but also with improved visuo-spatial perception (length estimation) due to increased self-identification with elements of the apparently retracting room allowing at the same time for a more vivid and realistic experience of those portions of the interior perceived in extra-personal space. Further studies could reveal if these measurable changes in spatial perception may be associated with self-location and an experienced displacement into the virtual interior.

Architecture from the First-Person Perspective

Linear perspective, if applied to immersive VR, allows to visually mimic the observer's first-person perspective by encoding subjective bodily parameters as viewpoint (body size and location), vanishing point (gaze direction), and monocular cues (i.e., size, line convergence, foreshortening, occlusion, texture, luminance, motion, and aerial perspective; Gibson, 1979; Cutting, 1997). In our VR setup the only cues to determine the relative dimension of room-size, were the virtual body filmed by the distanced camera in combination with perspective convergence in the narrow room, or, the visibility of the front wall in the horizontal direction in the large room. In the narrow condition the front wall was partly occluded by the virtual body (Figures 1C,D) and the sidewalls were more convergent toward the virtual body. In the large room condition, the virtual body and the front wall represented the most important spatial cues.

For both rooms, we did not observe any significant improvement of length estimation due to stroking for the bars presented horizontally on the front wall. This may be due to the higher visibility of the bars, allowing a more direct comparison with the reference bar. Accordingly, the present results in length estimation seem also to be compatible with the reported forward displacement in self-location that is absent along the right-left axis (Lenggenhager et al., 2007; Aspell et al., 2009). Thus, Lenggenhager et al. (2007) and Lenggenhager et al. (2009) found changes in the forward (or sagittal) direction, but not toward the right or the left. In the present setup the sagittal direction of the effects generally associated with the FBI also seems to be present in length estimation in the narrow room and only influence the latter in the sagittal direction.

Architectonic and Bodily Space

Interior space conveys a perceptual experience that is fundamental for architecture. Since ancient times this experience has been related to the phenomenology of space assumedly occurring through embodiment of a structural mass or spatial modules (Vitruvius, 1st century BC; Alberti, 1450; Semper, 1860). Crucial to the theories of architectonic embodiment is therefore the phenomenological transposition of the experience of architectonic space toward the masses or voids, hence translating own-body perception into architectonic dimensions. The outcome of our study may be discussed with respect to Heinrich Wölfflin's and August Schmarsow's notions of embodiment. In Heinrich Wölfflin's theory tectonic expressivity (i.e., achieved through crafted elements) facilitates embodiment with respect to the architectonic environment (Wölfflin, 1886). The arousal of characteristic architectonic sensations (e.g., familiarity, safety, containment, infinity, etc.) and “moods” (e.g., atmospheres) seems therefore to be evoked through an empathic resonance that the built environment induces within the human body. Hereby the structural moments of architectonic articulation can be felt by empathic observers, related to bodily physiognomy (e.g., through orientation, verticality, and symmetry) and to own-body processing.

Early theories of empathy (or sympathetic projection; Vischer, 1872; Lotze, 1884) mention that oculomotor mechanisms induce a bodily resonance within form and space, particularly through a transposition of perspective toward the figurative mass. In Wölfflin's interpretation, architectonic experience is not only linked to oculomotor (and potentially vestibular mechanisms), but architectonic form also resonates with mechanisms related to the bodily boundaries, in terms of a haptic (i.e., tactile, proprioceptive, and motor) response to visual and vestibular stimuli (Wölfflin, 1886, 1915). Crucially, stressing links to bodily processing, Wölfflin mentions that architectonic asymmetry would have comparable effects on the human body “as if a limb was missing” (Wölfflin, 1886).

August Schmarsow's reply to this proposition points beyond Vischer's and Wölfflin's visuo-motor and physiological mechanisms (Schmarsow, 1893). He speculates about a human sense of space historically developed in the interiors of primordial inhabitations and related to somesthetic projections. In his terms, the sense of space is based on the somesthetic experience of the architectonic interiors and on an “objectified” perspective gained by a perspective transposed toward the interior void. Schmarsow's observer can thus be characterized as being positioned inside an architectonic void, experiencing the interior from within (and not from in front as the observer in Wölfflin's theory). Furthermore, through self-displacements within the void, the sense of space seems to bear an objectified notion, through which previous points of self-location may be identified from several locations, thus progressively generating higher-level perspective representations through space.

By highlighting aspects of bodily self-consciousness in the perception of architectonic space, including motor aspects, both, Wölfflin and Schmarsow, highlighted experiences related to bodily processing for the understanding and the appreciations of architecture. Our empirical data strengthen this link and indicate a potential relation between architectonic theory and bodily self-consciousness (Blanke and Metzinger, 2009). The present participants experienced differences in illusory touch and room retraction depending on room-size, suggestive of a mild self-identification not only with the virtual body, but also with the walls through an involvement of somatosensory mechanisms, compatible with suggestions by Wölfflin and Schmarsow.

Our behavioral data show that in the narrow room such architectonic embodiment, probably modulated through the sidewalls, concerned not only the tectonic elements (walls) but the whole architectonic interior, as the length stimuli presented in the direction of the perspective of our participants were more realistically estimated in the FBI. Illusory touch with the sidewalls may be in line with Wölfflin's notion of resonance, whereas self-identification with the virtual interior in combination with improved size estimation after the FBI in the narrow room might relate to a projection of bodily space toward the architectonic void, and, of the bodily boundaries toward the enclosure. Schmarsow's observer—being positioned within the architecture and experiencing the space from a first-person perspective oriented toward the void—could therefore be related to experimentally induced changes of bodily self-consciousness of the observer through the FBI in immersive VR.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Nicole Ottiger for her photographs of the experimental setup and Roy Salomon for proofreading and precious suggestions on the manuscript. This study was supported by the grant Nr. 100013_130358 of the Swiss National Foundation.

References

Alberti, L. B. (1450). De re Aedificatoria (On the Art of Building in Ten Books). Cambridge: The MIT Press (1988).

Argan, G. C. (1946). The architecture of brunelleschi and the origins of perspective theory in the fifteenth century. J. Warburg Courtauld Inst. 9, 99–121. doi: 10.2307/750311

CrossRef Full Text

Armel, K., and Ramachandran, V. (2003). Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 1499–1506. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2364

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aspell, J. E., Lenggenhager, B., and Blanke, O. (2009). Keeping in touch with one's self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness. PLoS ONE 4:e6488. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006488

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanke, O., and Metzinger, T. (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burckhardt, J. (1860). The Civilization of the Renaissance in Italy. London: Middlemore (1878).

Craven, B. J. (1993). Orientation dependence of human line-length judgments matches statistical structure in real-world scenes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 253, 101–106. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1993.0087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cutting, J. E. (1997). How the eye measures reality and virtual reality. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 29, 27–36. doi: 10.3758/BF03200563

CrossRef Full Text

Ehrsson, H. H. (2007). The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science 317, 1048. doi: 10.1126/science.1142175

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Luppino, G., Matelli, M., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). J. Neurophysiol. 76, 141–157.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. New York, NY: Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis (1986).

Haggard, P., Christakou, A., and Serino, A. (2007). Viewing the body modulates tactile receptive fields. Exp. Brain Res. 180, 187–193. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0971-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hohwy, J., and Paton, B. (2010). Explaining away the body: experiences of supernaturally caused touch and touch on non-hand objects within the rubber hand illusion. PLoS ONE 5:e9416. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009416

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Howe, C. Q., and Purves, D. (2002). Range image statistics can explain the anomalous perception of length. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 13184–13188. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162474299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Howe, C. Q., and Purves, D. (2005). Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and line orientation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 1228–1233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409311102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kannape, O. A., Schwabe, L., Tadi, T., and Blanke, O. (2010). The limits of agency in walking humans. Neuropsychologia 48, 1628–1636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ladavas, E., Di Pellegrino, G., Farne, A., and Zeloni, G. (1998). Neuropsychological evidence of an integrated visuotactile representation of peripersonal space in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 581–589. doi: 10.1162/089892998562988

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lenggenhager, B., Mouthon, M., and Blanke, O. (2009). Spatial aspects of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., and Blanke, O. (2007). Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science 317, 1096–1099. doi: 10.1126/science.1143439

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Linkenauger, S. A., Ramenzoni, V., and Proffitt, D. R. (2010). Illusory shrinkage and growth: body-based rescaling affects the perception of size. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1318–1325. doi: 10.1177/0956797610380700

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lotze, H. (1884). Grundzüge der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Hirzel.

Mohler, B. J., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Thompson, W. B., and Bulthoff, H. H. (2010). The effect of viewing a self-avatar on distance judgments in an HMD-based virtual environment. Presence Teleoper. Virtual Environ. 19, 230–242. doi: 10.1162/pres.19.3.230

CrossRef Full Text

Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philos. Psychol. 1, 35–59. doi: 10.1080/09515088808572924

CrossRef Full Text

Petkova, V. I., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS ONE 3:e3832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003832

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science 277, 190–191. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5323.190

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., and Gentilucci, M. (1981). Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behav. Brain Res. 2, 147–163. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salomon, R., Van Elk, M., Aspell, J., and Blanke, O. (2012). I feel who I see: visual body identity affects visual–tactile integration in peripersonal space. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1355–1364. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sambo, C. F., and Forster, B. (2009). An ERP investigation on visuotactile interactions in peripersonal and extrapersonal space: evidence for the spatial rule. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1550–1559. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmarsow, A. (1893). “The Essence of Architectural Creation,” in Empathy, Form and Space–Problems in German Aesthetics 1873–1893, ed H. F. Mallgrave (Santa Monica, CA: The Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities, 1994), 125–148.

Schwabe, L., and Blanke, O. (2008). The vestibular component in out-of-body experiences: a computational approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2:17. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.017.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Semper, G. (1860). Style in the Technical and Tectonic Arts; or, Practical Aesthetics. Los Angeles, CA: The Getty Research Institute, (2004).

Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Front. Neurosci. 3, 214–220. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.029.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teneggi, C., Canzoneri, E., di Pellegrino, G., and Serino, A. (2013). Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Curr. Biol. 23, 406–411. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tsakiris, M., Hesse, M. D., Boy, C., Haggard, P., and Fink, G. R. (2007). Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2235–2244. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl131

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van der Hoort, B., Guterstam, A., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). Being Barbie: the size of one's own body determines the perceived size of the world. PLoS ONE 6:e20195. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vischer, R. (1872). “On the optical sense of form: a contribution to aesthetics,” in Empathy, Form and Space–Problems in German Aesthetics 1873–1893, ed H. F. Mallgrave (Santa Monica, CA: The Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities, 1994), 89–123.

Vitruvius, M. P. (1st century BC). De Architectura Libri Decem (Zehn Bücher über Architektur). Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft (1891, 2008).

Witt, J. K., and Proffitt, D. R. (2007). Perceived slant: a dissociation between perception and action. Perception 36, 249–257. doi: 10.1068/p5449

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., and Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 880–888. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.880

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wölfflin, H. (1886). “Prolegomena to a psychology of architecture,” in Empathy, Form and Space–Problems in German Aesthetics 1873–1893, ed H. F. Mallgrave (Santa Monica, CA: The Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities, 1994), 125–148.

Wölfflin, H. (1915). Principles of Art History. The Problem of the Development of Style in Later Art. New York, NY: Dover Publications (1932).

Keywords: bodily self-consciousness, embodiment, architecture, virtual reality, length estimation

Citation: Pasqualini I, Llobera J and Blanke O (2013) “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: how bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors. Front. Psychol. 4:354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00354

Received: 08 December 2012; Accepted: 30 May 2013;
Published online: 25 June 2013.

Edited by:

Kathleen T. Ashenfelter, US Census Bureau, USA

Reviewed by:

Kathleen T. Ashenfelter, US Census Bureau, USA
Heiko Hecht, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, Germany

Copyright © 2013 Pasqualini, Llobera and Blanke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Isabella Pasqualini, Atelier de la Conception de l'Espace, School of Architecture, Civil and Environmental Engineering, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Station 16, CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland e-mail: isabella.pasqualini@epfl.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.