Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Avoiding bias in medical ethical decision-making. Lessons to be learnt from psychology research

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When ethical decisions have to be taken in critical, complex medical situations, they often involve decisions that set the course for or against life-sustaining treatments. Therefore the decisions have far-reaching consequences for the patients, their relatives, and often for the clinical staff. Although the rich psychology literature provides evidence that reasoning may be affected by undesired influences that may undermine the quality of the decision outcome, not much attention has been given to this phenomenon in health care or ethics consultation. In this paper, we aim to contribute to the sensitization of the problem of systematic reasoning biases by showing how exemplary individual and group biases can affect the quality of decision-making on an individual and group level. We are addressing clinical ethicists as well as clinicians who guide complex decision-making processes of ethical significance. Knowledge regarding exemplary group psychological biases (e.g. conformity bias), and individual biases (e.g. stereotypes), will be taken from the disciplines of social psychology and cognitive decision science and considered in the field of ethical decision-making. Finally we discuss the influence of intuitive versus analytical (systematical) reasoning on the validity of ethical decision-making.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Binge eating disorder is characterized by compulsive overeating in which people consume huge amounts of food while feeling out of control and powerless to stop.

  2. Balancing of interest’ is a method developed in the discipline of ethics. It describes the process of practical reasoning needed for a qualified solution to a choice or decision conflict. Such conflicts arise because a specific decision may be beneficial for one person or group, but detrimental for another person or group. In such cases, the expected benefits and burdens must be weighed as carefully as possible, while taking into account individual and collective needs in addition to their urgency; the rights of the individual; precedence of public welfare, and questions of justice.

References

  • Aberegg, S.K., E.F. Haponik, and P.B. Terry. 2005. Omission bias and decision making in pulmonary and critical care medicine. Chest American College of Chest Physicians 128: 1497–1505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Agich, G. 2001. The question of method in ethics consultation. The American Journal of Bioethics 1(4): 31–41.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Albisser Schleger, H., and S. Reiter-Theil. 2007. ‘Age’ and ‘Costs’—Factors in treatment decisions at the end-of life? An analysis of informal knowledge structures of doctors and nurses. Ethik in der Medizin 19: 103–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albisser Schleger, H., H. Pargger, and S. Reiter-Theil. 2008. ‘Futility’—Overtreatment at the end of life? Reasons for missed cessations of therapy in geriatric and critical care. Zeitschrift für Palliativmedizin 9: 67–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Society for Bioethics and Humanities (ASBH). 1998. Core competences for health care ethics consultation. Reprinted 2003 Glenview, IL, USA. In Ethics consultation. From theory to practice, ed. M.P. Aulisio, R.M. Arnold, and S.J. Youngner, 165–209. Baltimore and London: The John Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C.J. 2003. The psychology of doing nothing—Forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin 129: 9–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S.E. 1956. Studies of independence and conformity—A minority of one against an unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs 70(9): 416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, D.A. 2001. Economics and decision science. In Methods in medical ethics, ed. J. Sugarman, and D.P. Sulmasy, 227–234. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S., A. van de Loo, and S. Reiter-Theil. 2008. A ‘little bit illegal’? Withholding and withdrawing of mechanical ventilation in the eyes of German intensive care physicians. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 11: 7–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beecher, H.K. 1955. The powerful placebo. The Journal of the American Medicine Association 159: 1602–1606.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal, E.W. 2000. Errors in ethics consultation. In Margin of error: The ethics of mistakes in the practice of medicine, ed. S.B. Rubin, and L. Zoloth, 255–272. Hagerstown, Maryland: University Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierhoff, H. 2006. Sozialpsychologie—Ein Lehrbuch. Stuttgart: W. Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, H. 1997. The doctor as therapeutic agent: A placebo research agenda. In The placebo effect—An interdisciplinary exploration, ed. E. Harrington, 77–92. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J.D., and P.J. Fairey. 1989. Informational and normative routes to conformity—The effect of faction size as a function on norm extremity and attention to the stimulus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57: 457–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. 2005. The theory and practice of clinical decision-making. The Canadian Journal of Anasthesia 52(6/pp): R1–R8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D. 1997. The logic of failure: Recognizing and avoiding error in complex situations. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J., R.J. Boyleand, and E.M. Spencer. 2000. Errors in Healthcare Ethics Consultation. In Margin of error: The ethics of mistakes in the practice of medicine, ed. S.B. Rubin, and L. Zoloth, 343–373. Hagerstown, Maryland: University Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foerde, R., R. Pedersen, and V. Akre. 2008. Clinicians’ evaluation of clinical ethics consultations in Norway: A tool for quality improvement. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 11(1): 17–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerard, H.B., R.A. Wilhelmy, and E.S. Conolley. 1968. Conformity and group size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 8: 79–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., P.M. Todd, and The ABC Group. 1999. Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigone, D., and R. Hastie. 1993. The common knowledge effect: Information sharing and group judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 959–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigone, D., and R. Hastie. 1996. The impact of information on group judgment: A model and computer simulation. In Understanding group behavior: Consensual action by small groups, Vol. 1, ed. E. Witte, and J.H. Davis, 221–251. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. 1995. Moral consciousness and communicative action. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R., and P.M. Todd. 2003. More is not always better—The benefits of cognitive limits. In Thinking—Psychological perspectives on reasoning, judgment and decision making, ed. D. Hardman, and L. Macchi, 213–231. Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. 1978. A treatise of human nature. Clarendon: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurst, S.A., R. Forde, S. Reiter-Theil, A.M. Slowther, A. Perrier, R. Pegoraro, and M. Danis. 2007. Physicians’ views on resource availability and equity in four European health care systems. BMC Health Services Research 7: 137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janis, I.L. 1982. Groupthink—Psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoe, 2nd ed. Boston: Hougthon Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jungermann, H., H.R. Pfister, and K. Fischer. 2005. Die Psychologie der Entscheidung—Eine Einführung. Heidelberg: Spektrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H.H. 1955. The two functions of reference groups. In Readings in social psychology, ed. G.E. Swanson, T.E. Newcomb, and E.L. Hartley, 410–414. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J.R., and S.J. Karau. 1999. Group decision making—The effects of initial preferences and time pressure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25: 1342–1354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L., C. Levine, and A. Hewer. 1983. Moral stages: A current formulation and a response to critics. Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, J.R.J., C. Christensen, T.M. Franz, and A.S. Abbott. 1998. Diagnosing groups—The pooling, management, and impact of shared and unshared case information in team-based medical decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75: 93–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M.J. 1980. The belief in a just world—A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. 1974. Obedience to authority–An experimental view. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. 1976. Obedience to criminal orders—The compulsion to do evil. In Contemporary social psychology—Representative readings, ed. T. Blass, 175–184. Itasca: Peacock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molewijk, B., M. Verkerk, H. Milius, and G. Widdershoven. 2008. Implementing moral case deliberation in a psychiatric hospital: Process and outcome. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 11(1): 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G.E. 1959. Principia ethica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunner-Winkler, G. 1984. Two Moralities? A Critical Discussion of an Ethic of Care and Responsibility versus an Ethic of Rights and Justice. In Morality, moral behavior, and moral development, ed. W.M. Kurtines, and J.L. Gewirtz. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, C.D., and R.A. Cowlin. 1996. Acceptance of uncommon information into group discussion when that information is or is not demonstrable. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 66(3): 307–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfaefflin, M., K. Kobert, and S. Reiter-Theil. 2009. Evaluating clinical ethics consultation. Cambridge Quarterly Healthcare Ethics 18(4): 406–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, J. 1971. A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeier, D.A., L.E. Ferris, J.V. Tu, J.E. Hux, and M.J. Schull. 2001. Problems for clinical judgement—Introducing cognitive psychology as one more basic science. Journal of Applied Mathematics and Computing 164(3): 358–360.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reitemeier, P.J. 2000. Quality and error in bioethics consultation: A puzzle in pieces. In Margin of error: The ethics of mistakes in the practice of medicine, ed. S.B. Rubin, and L. Zoloth, 231–253. Hagerstown, Maryland: University Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Theil, S. 2001. Ethics consultation in Germany: The present situation. HEC-Forum 13(3): 265–280.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Theil, S. 2003. Balancing the Perspectives. The patient’s role in clinical ethics consultation. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 6: 247–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Theil, S. 2009. Dealing with the normative dimension in clinical ethics consultation. Cambridge Quarterly Healthcare Ethics 18(4): 347–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Theil, S., M. Mertz, and B. Meyer-Zehnder. 2007. The complex Role of Relatives in end-of-life decision-making: An ethical analysis. HEC-Forum 19(4): 341–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, G. 2007. Greater patient, family and surrogate involvement in clinical ethics consultation: The model of clinical ethics liaison as a measure for preventive ethics. HEC-Forum 19(4): 327–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, L.A. 1961. Group size, prior experience and conformity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63: 436–437.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. 1966. Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York: Appleton-Entury-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, J., S. Ross, and B. McClung. 2006. Ethical decision making and organizational behavior: A case of life and death. HEC Forum 18: 193–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, S.B., and L. Zoloth (eds.). 2000. Margin of error: The ethics of mistakes in the practice of medicine. Hagerstown, Maryland: University Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saks, M.J. 1977. Jury verdicts—The role of group size and social decision rule. Lexington MA: Heath.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saks, M.J., and M.W. Marti. 1997. A meta-analysis of the effects of jury size. Law and Human Behavior 21(5): 451–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz-Hardt, S., F. Brodbeck, A. Mojzisch, R. Kerschreiter, and D. Frey. 2006. Group decision making in hidden profile situations—Dissent as a facilitator for decision quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 6: 1080–1093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spranca, M., E. Minsk, and J. Baron. 1991. Omission and commission in judgment and choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 27(1): 76–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stasser, G. 2000. Information distribution, participation and group decision—Explorations with the DISCUSS and SPEAK models. In Computational modelling of behaviour in organizations, ed. D.R. Ilgen, and C.L. Hulin, 135–161. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stasser, G., and W. Titus. 1985. Pooling of unshared information in group decision making—Biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48: 1467–1478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sugarman, J., and D.P. Sulmasy (eds.). 2001. Methods in medical ethics. Washington D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, C.R. 2005. Moral heuristics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(4): 531–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E.L. 1920. A constant error in psychological rating. Journal of Applied Psychology 4: 25–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zikmund-Fisher, B.J., B. Sarr, A. Fagerlin, and P.A. Ubel. 2006. A matter of perspective—Choosing for others differs from choosing for yourself in making treatment decisions. Journal of General Internal Medicine 21(6): 618–622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This paper is part of the project “Between Over-Treatment and Under-Treatment: Ethical Problems of Micro-Allocation”, no. 3200B0-113724/1, funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation. We are grateful for the helpful comments of Prof. Volker Dittmann, Legal and Forensic Medicine, University of Basel. We also thank Marcel Mertz, MA, for his helpful comments. Piera Jones, MA, and Nicola Stingelin Giles, MBA, MAS, MAE gave invaluable help in translating and editing the manuscript into native English. The paper has benefited much from the discussions in the PhD Colloquium, Dept. Medical and Health Ethics, Medical Faculty, University Hospital Basel.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heidi Albisser Schleger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Albisser Schleger, H., Oehninger, N.R. & Reiter-Theil, S. Avoiding bias in medical ethical decision-making. Lessons to be learnt from psychology research. Med Health Care and Philos 14, 155–162 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-010-9263-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-010-9263-2

Keywords

Navigation