Skip to main content
Log in

Feeling good vibrations in dialogical relations

  • Published:
Continental Philosophy Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

I engage phenomenological and empirical perspectives on dialogical relations in infancy in a mutually enlightening and challenging relation. On the one hand, the empirical contributions provide evidence for the primacy of first-to-second person interrelatedness in human sociality, as opposed to the claim of primary syncretism heralded by Merleau-Ponty, and also in distinction from the ego-alter ego model routinely used in phenomenology. On the other hand, phenomenological considerations regarding the lived affective experience of dialogical relatedness enrich and render intelligible the psychological accounts of dialogue in terms of observable behavior. Phenomenological and empirical perspectives on dialogical relatedness thus combine to offer an affectively charged and conversationally patterned notion of primary intersubjectivity in the I-you mode.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Merleau-Ponty (2000, p. 25).

  2. Ibid., 12.

  3. Gallagher (2003).

  4. Varela (1996).

  5. Dennett (1991). I discuss this point further in Stawarska (2003).

  6. For a discussion of Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenological account of intersubjectivity in a mutually constraining and enlightening relation to the empirical studies on neonate imitation in developmental psychology, cf. Stawarska “Merleau-Ponty in Dialogue with the Cognitive Sciences in Light of Recent Imitation Research,” op cit.

  7. Merleau-Ponty (1964).

  8. Guillaume (1973).

  9. Wallon (1993).

  10. Lacan (1982).

  11. Merleau-Ponty op cit., 220.

  12. Meltzoff and Moore (1977; 1989; 1994).

  13. Stawarska (2004).

  14. Merleau-Ponty (1962).

  15. Husserl (1970).

  16. I discuss this idea further in Stawarska (2007) and in a book manuscript under review.

  17. Trevarthen (1979, 1993).

  18. Bateson (1975).

  19. Lyons (1977, p. 637).

  20. Spitz (1963).

  21. Cf., e.g., Bateson. “Mother–infant Exchanges,” op cit.; Beebe (1979), Papousek and Papousek (1979), Stern (1974), Stern et al. (1977).

  22. Kaye and Wells (1980).

  23. Jaffe and Feldstein (1970).

  24. As presented in Jaffe et al. (2001). There may be slight differences in the parsing models used by the researchers in the field.

  25. Jaffe and Feldstein, The Rhythms of Dialogue, op cit., 45.

  26. Cassotta et al. (1967), quoted in Jaffe et al., Rhythms of Dialogue in Infancy, op cit., 42.

  27. Jaffe and Feldstein, The Rhythms of Dialogue, op cit.

  28. Feldstein and Welkowitz (1978).

  29. Bakeman and Brown (1977).

  30. Cf., e.g., Beebe, Stern and Jaffe, “The Kinesic Rhythm of Mother-infant Interactions,” op cit.; Beebe et al. (1985); Stern (1974) and Stern et al. “The Infant’s Stimulus World During Social Interaction,” op cit.

  31. Beebe et al. (1988).

  32. Jasnow and Feldstein (1986).

  33. Ibid., p. 757.

  34. Ibid.

  35. Beebe et al., “Vocal Congruence in Mother-infant Play,” op cit.; Beatrice Beebe et al., “Interpersonal Timing,” op cit.; Jasnow and Feldstein, “Adult-like Temporal Characteristics of Mother–infant Vocal Interactions,” op cit.

  36. Cf., e.g., Rochat (1999).

  37. My thanks to Brady Heiner for raising the question about the evidence for bidirectional matching, and the need to consider infant–infant interaction to settle the question of dialogical primacy.

  38. Daniel Stern et al., “The Infant’s Stimulus World During Social Interaction,” op cit.

  39. Trevarthen (1993).

  40. Jaffe et al., Rhythms of Dialogue in Infancy, op cit.

  41. Rochat, “Dialogical Nature of Cognition,” in Jaffe et al., Rhythms of Dialogue in Infancy, op cit.

  42. Crown (1991).

  43. Feldstein and Welkowitz, “A Chronography of Conversation,” op cit.

  44. Jaffe et al., Rhythms of Dialogue in Infancy, op cit.

  45. Tanz (1980, p. 163).

  46. Gallagher (2001).

  47. Rochat, “Dialogical Nature of Cognition,” op cit., 140.

  48. Vygotsky (1978).

  49. Jaffe and Anderson (1979).

  50. Beebe et al., “Interpersonal Timing,” op cit.; Stern, “Mother and Infant at Play,” op cit.; Stern et al., “The Infant’s Stimulus World During Social Interaction,” op cit.

  51. Merleau-Ponty, Phenomenology of Perception, op cit., 225.

  52. Ibid., p. 217.

  53. The idea that gesture and speech are inextricably interrelated has recently received substantial reinforcement from the leading researcher in the field. Cf., for example, McNeill (2005).

References

  • Bakeman, Roger, and Josephine Brown. 1977. Behavioral dialogues: An approach to the assessment of mother-infant interaction. Child Development 48: 195–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, Mary Catherine. 1975. Mother-infant exchanges: The epigenesist of conversational interaction. In Developmental psycholinguistics and communication disorders. Annals of the New York academy of sciences 263, ed. Doris Aaronson and Robert W. Rieber. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

  • Beebe, Beatrice, Danie Stern, and Joseph Jaffe. 1979. The kinesic rhythm of mother-infant interactions. In Of speech and time: Temporal patterns in interpersonal contexts, ed. Aron W. Siegman and Stanley Feldstein. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  • Beebe, Beatrice, Joseph Jaffe, Stanley Feldstein, K. Mays, and Diane Alson. 1985. Interpersonal timing: The application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinesic interactions. In Social perception in infants, ed. Tiffany Field and Nathan Fox. Norwood: Ablex.

  • Beebe, Beatrice, Diane Alson, Joseph Jaffe, Stanley Feldstein, and Cynthia Crown. 1988. Vocal congruence in mother-infant play. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 17(3): 245–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassotta, Louis, Stanley Feldstein, and Joseph Jaffe. 1967. The stability and modifiability of individual vocal characteristics in stress and nonstress interviews. Research Bulletin No. 2. New York: The William Alanson White Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crown, Cynthia. 1991. Coordinated interpersonal timing of vision and voice as a function of interpersonal attraction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 10(1): 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, Daniel. 1991. Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, Daniel. 2001. The Fantasy of First-person Science http://ase.tufts.edu/cogstud/pubpage.htm.

  • Feldstein, Stanley, and Joan Welkowitz. 1978. A chronography of conversation: In defense of an objective approach. In Nonverbal behavior and communication, ed. Aron.W. Siegman and Stanley Feldstein. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  • Gallagher, Shaun. 2001. The practice of mind: Theory, simulation of primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5–7): 83–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, Shaun. 2003. Phenomenology and experimental design: Toward a phenomenologically enlightened experimental science. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(9–10): 85–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, Shaun, and Andrew Meltzoff. 1996. The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology 9(2): 213–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guillaume, Paul. 1973. La formation des habitudes chez l’enfant. Presses Universitaires de France.

  • Husserl, Edmund. 1970. Crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology (trans: David Carr). Evanston: Northwestern.

  • Jaffe, Joseph, and Samuel W. Anderson. 1979. Communication rhythms and the evolution of language. In Of speech and time: Temporal speech patterns in interpersonal contexts, ed. Aron W. Siegman and Stanley Feldstein. Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates.

  • Jaffe, Joseph, Beatrice Beebe, Stanley Feldstein, Cynthia Crown, and Michael Jasnow. 2001. Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 66(2).

  • Jaffe, Joseph, and Stanley Feldstein. 1970. The rhythms of dialogue. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasnow, Michael, and Stanley Feldstein. 1986. Adult-like temporal characteristics of mother-infant vocal interactions. Child Development 57: 754–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K., and A. Wells. 1980. Mothers’ jiggling and the burst-pause pattern in neonatal sucking. Infant Behavior and Development 3: 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lacan, Jacques. 1982. The mirror stage as formative of the function of the i as revealed in psychoanalytic experience. Écrits. W.W. Norton & Co.

  • Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics, Vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, David. 2005. Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Keith Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Keith Moore. 1989. Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology 25: 954–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Keith Moore. 1994. Imitation, memory and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development 17: 83–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, Maurice. 1962. Phenomenology of perception. (trans: Colin Smith). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  • Merleau-Ponty, Maurice. 1964. The child’s relations with others. (trans: William Cobb). In The Primacy of Perception. Evanston: Northwestern.

  • Merleau-Ponty, Maurice. 2000. Titres et travaux: Project d’enseignement. In Parcours II, 1951–1961. Verdier.

  • Papousek, H., and M. Papousek. 1979. Early ontogeny of human social interaction. In Human ethology: Claims and limits of a new discipline, ed. Mario von Cranach, Klaus Foppa, W. Lepenies, and D. Ploog. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, Phillipe. 1999. Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Rochat, Phillipe. 2001. Dialogical nature of cognition. In rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 66(2): 133–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, René A. 1963. The evolution of the dialogue. In Drives, affects, and behavior, ed. Max Schur, Vol. 2. New York: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stawarska, Beata. 2003. Merleau-Ponty in dialogue with the cognitive sciences in light of recent imitation research. Philosophy Today 47(5): 89–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stawarska, Beata. 2004. Anonymity and sociality: The convergence of psychological and philosophical currents in Merleau-Ponty’s ontological theory of intersubjectivity. CHIASMI International 5: 295–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stawarska, Beata. 2007. Persons, pronouns, and perspectives: Linguistic and developmental contributions to dialogical phenomenology. In Folk psychology reassessed, ed. Daniel Hutto and Matthew Ratcliffe. Dordrecht: Springer.

  • Stern, Daniel. 1974. Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal and gaze behaviors. In The effect of the infant on its caregiver, ed. Michael Lewis and Leonard Rosenblum. New York: Wiley.

  • Stern, Daniel. 2001. Face-to-face play: Its temporal structure as predictor of socioaffective development. In rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 66(2): 144–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, Daniel, Beatrice Beebe, Joseph Jaffe, and S. Bennett. 1977. The infant’s stimulus world during social interaction. In Studies in mother-infant interaction, ed. H. Rudolph Schaffer. New York: Academic Press.

  • Tanz, Christine. 1980. Studies in the acquisition of deictic terms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. Margaret Bullowa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1993. The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In Ecological and interpersonal knowledge of self, ed. Ulric Neisser. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Varela, Francisco. 1996. Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy to the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 3: 330–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, Lev. 1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. In ed. Michael Cole, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

  • Wallon, Henri. 1993. Les origins du caractere chez l’enfant. Presses Universitaires de France.

Download references

Acknowledgements

I wish to thank the editor of this issue, Brady Heiner, and the anonymous reviewers, for helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this essay.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beata Stawarska.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stawarska, B. Feeling good vibrations in dialogical relations. Cont Philos Rev 41, 217–236 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11007-008-9079-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11007-008-9079-4

Keywords

Navigation