Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 99, Issue 1, February 2006, Pages 53-71
Cognition

What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apes

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.005Get rights and content

Abstract

The study investigates what an intermediate success rate means in bonobos, chimpanzees, and orangutans. Apes participated in liquid conservation experiments where they had to track the larger of two different quantities of juice after various kinds of transformations [Suda, C., & Call, J. (2004). Piagetian liquid conservation in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 265–279). When making a decision, apes sometimes demonstrated hesitant behavior, concurrently pointing to both alternatives or successively changing their choice. Moderately successful apes showed more hesitation than highly successful or unsuccessful apes. The results are consistent with the cognitive conflict model: The experiments created a higher degree of cognitive conflict on moderately successful apes than on very successful or unsuccessful apes. This indicates that an intermediate performance reflects the joint operation and potential conflict between two different cognitive strategies (identity and appearance) inherent to the Piagetian conservation task.

Section snippets

Cognitive conflict model

Subjects will show more hesitation as the degree of cognitive conflict increases. Consequently, the model predicts that subjects using a single strategy will show less hesitation than subjects that are shifting between two strategies. The more heavily apes rely on a single strategy (and the closer the apes' success rate nears either extreme), the less hesitant they will become. Therefore, this model predicts an inverted U-shaped curve between success and hesitation. A 75% success rate will

Subjects

Four bonobos, five chimpanzees, and five orangutans served as subjects in Experiment 1, while the same subjects except for one chimpanzee (Sandra), who refused to take part in the testing, participated in Experiments 2 and 3. Table 1 provides further descriptions of the subjects used in this study.

Task procedure

The task was originally designed for investigating the apes' understanding of Piagetian liquid conservation (Suda & Call, 2004). The apes were tested in indoor cages with a Plexiglas window that had a

Results

There were no significant species differences in the apes' hesitation score across the experiments. An ANOVA on the apes' hesitation score with experiment as a within-subjects factor and species as a between-subjects factor revealed no effect of experiment, F(2,20)=0.03, P=.970, species, F(2,10)=1.33, P=.307, or an interaction between the two factors, F(4,20)=0.09, P=.985. The distribution of the subjects' data points on the y-axis could not be explained by species differences. Therefore, we

Discussion

The cognitive conflict model best fit the relationship between the apes' hesitation and their success rate. Moderately successful apes tended to show more hesitant behavior than very successful or unsuccessful apes in our series of liquid conservation experiments. Note that the other two models that we considered failed to explain our data satisfactorily. The simple monitoring of reward gain predicts that apes will hesitate more as the amount of their reward gain decreases, which we did not

Acknowledgements

Applicable animal care and use rules and guidelines were followed in the conduct of this research. We thank the staff of the Wolfgang Köhler Primate Research Center for their support. We also thank Daniel Stahl for statistical advice and Friederike Schmitz for helping with reliability assessment. C.S. would like to thank Laurence King for his useful comments on the original version of this manuscript, and Keith Jensen for grammatical correction. A part of this study was presented at the COE 21

References (25)

  • G.N. Cantor

    Conflict, learning, and piaget: Comments on Zimmerman and Blom's “Toward an empirical test of the role of cognitive conflict in learning”

    Developmental Review

    (1983)
  • J.D. Smith et al.

    The uncertain response in humans and animals

    Cognition

    (1997)
  • M.J. Beran

    Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

    Journal of Comparative Psychology

    (2001)
  • J. Call

    On linking comparative metacognition and theory of mind

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2003)
  • Call, J. (2005). The self and the other: A missing link in comparative social cognition. In H. Terrace, J. Metcalfe...
  • J. Call et al.

    Do apes and children know what they have seen?

    Animal Cognition

    (2001)
  • J. Call et al.

    Chimpanzee gaze following in an object-choice task

    Animal Cognition

    (1998)
  • J. Call et al.

    Liquid conservation in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and humans (Homo sapiens): Individual differences and perceptual strategies

    Journal of Comparative Psychology

    (1996)
  • J. Call et al.

    Perceptual strategies in the estimation of physical quantities by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus)

    Journal of Comparative Psychology

    (1997)
  • R.R. Hampton

    Rhesus monkeys know when they remember

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

    (2001)
  • Hampton, R. R. (2005). Can rhesus monkeys discriminate between remembering and forgetting? In H. Terrace, J. Metcalfe...
  • R.R. Hampton et al.

    Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting

    Animal Cognition

    (2004)
  • Cited by (16)

    • Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) show subtle signs of uncertainty when choices are more difficult

      2021, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, relating these findings to studies of wavering and metacognition in humans is complicated by the facts that hesitation scores in both studies collapsed rather different types of motor behaviors, and that the relationship between hesitation and difficulty was based on subjects' aggregated performances on the level of individuals or test conditions, respectively. For example, Suda and Call (2006) investigated whether great apes' hesitation was related to performance in a Piagetian liquid conservation task. Wavering back and forth between options, and the simultaneous picking of two options with both hands, were both collapsed in a single measure of “hesitation”.

    • Future Thinking: Children But Not Apes Consider Multiple Possibilities

      2016, Current Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous research has shown that great apes can take multiple courses of action in tasks where they are uncertain. Suda and Call [16] reported that apes faced with uncertainty about which of two cups contained the greater reward would use both hands and point to both cups. Conversely, an experiment by Beck et al. [17] showed that children can represent alternative states of reality without spontaneously preparing for multiple possibilities.

    • When in doubt, chimpanzees rely on estimates of past reward amounts

      2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text