Skip to main content
Log in

“Questions” in Argument Sequences in Japanese

  • Published:
Human Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study reports on the use of a linguistic category "interrogative," which has been traditionally associated with the act of questioning, and its use in argument talk in Japanese. Based on the observation that interrogative utterances in argument data are regularly followed by non-answers, it is argued that interrogative utterances in argument sequences may not be designed/interpreted as doing questioning. Such use of interrogatives can become an orderly practice to which participants orient themselves in social activities recognizable as arguments. However, though an answer is not expected, the recipient invariably provides some form of response, or the initial speaker seeks such a response when none is provided. Thus the nature of interrogatives as a grammatical category seems to reside in the basic structural unit of social interaction [recipient-oriented action]-[response]. In general, this study is intended to show the dynamically interlocking relationship between grammar and interaction by exploring the intricate interplay between a local action for which interrogative grammar is employed, and the sequential environment and activity framework in which the action takes place.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Athanasiadou, A. (1991). The discourse functions of questions.Pragmatics 1(1): 107-122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J.M. and Drew, P. (1979). Order in Court: The Organization of Verbal Interaction in Judicial Settings.London: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J.M. and Heritage, J. (Eds.) (1984). Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulter, J. (1990). Elementary properties of argument sequences.In G. Psathas (Ed.), Interaction competence, pp. 181-203.Washington, D.C.: University Press of America and International Institute for Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulthard, M. (1985). An Introduction to Discourse Analysis.London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drew, P. (1984). Speaker's reportings in invitation sequences.In J.M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis, pp. 129-151.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J.W. (1985). Competing motivations.In J. Haiman (Ed.), Iconicity in Syntax, pp. 343-365.Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J.W. (1987). The discourse basis of ergativity.Language 63: 805-855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J.W. (1997). Lecture. Advanced discourse course.Winter, the University of California at Santa Barbara.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, B. (1994). Contextualization, indexicality, and the distributed nature of grammar.Language Sciences 16(1): 1-37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freed, A.F. (1994). The form and function of questions in informal dyadic conversation.Journal of Pragmatics 21: 621-644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M.H. (1990). He-Said-She-Said: Talk as Social Organization among Black Children.Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, A. (Ed.) (1990). Conflict Talk.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology.Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. and Roth, A.L. (1995). Grammar and institution: questions and questioning in the broadcast news interview.Research on Language and Social Interaction 28(1): 1-60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J. (1988). Emergent grammar and the a priori grammar principle.In D. Tannen (Ed.), Linguistics in Context: Connecting Observation and Understanding.117-134.Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P. and Thompson, S.A. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse.Language 56(2): 251-299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P. and Traugott, E.C. (1993). Grammaticalization.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamio, A. (1990). Johoo no nawabari riron [A theory of territory of information]. Tokyo: Taishuukan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotthoff, H. (1993). Disagreement and concession in disputes: On the context sensitivity of preference structures.Language in Society 22: 193-216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. and Fanshel, D. (1977). Therapeutic Discourse: Psychotherapy as Conversation.Academic Press.

  • Levinson, S.C. (1983). Pragmatics.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, D.W. (1985). How children start arguments.Language in Society 14: 1-29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, S.K. (1993). Kaiwa bunseki [Conversational Analysis]. Tokyo: Kuroshio.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, S.K. (1995). Interrogatives that seek no answers: exploring the expressiveness of rhetorical interrogatives in Japanese.Linguistics 33: 501-530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McHoul, A.W. (1987). Why there are no guarantees for interrogators.Journal of Pragmatics 11: 455-471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mori, J. (1996). Negotiating Agreement and Disagreement: The Uses of Connective Expressions in Japanese Conversations.Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin-Madison.

  • Nofsinger, R.E. (1991). Everyday Conversation.Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Schegloff, E.A. and Thompson, S.A. (Eds.) (1996). Interaction and Grammar.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, A.M. (1978). Attribution of responsibility: Blamings.Sociology 12: 115-21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, A.M. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes.In J.M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis, pp. 57-101.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, A.M. (1988). Offering a candidate answer: An information seeking strategy.Communication Monographs 55: 360-373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, R.G., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. and Svartvik, J. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language.New York: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1987). On the preferences for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation.In G. Bolton and J.R.E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and Social Organization, pp. 54- 69.Cleverdon, England: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A. and Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematic for the organization of turn-taking for conversation Language 50: 696-735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1972). Sequencing in conversational openings.In J.J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics, pp. 346-380.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1982). Discourse as and interactional achievement: some uses of 'uh huh' and other things that come between sentences.In D. Tannen (Ed.), Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk. Georgetown University Round Table on Language and Linguistics.Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1984). On some questions and ambiguities in conversation.In J.M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis, pp. 28-52.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1988). Presequences and indirection: applying speech act theory to ordinary conversation.Journal of Pragmatics 12: 55-62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1992). Repair after next turn: the last structurally Provided Defense of Intersubjectivity in conversation.American Journal of Sociology 97(5): 1295-1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. and H. Sacks. (1973). Opening up closings.Semiotica 7: 289-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J.R. (1969). Speech Acts.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, H. (1996). Language, Culture and Social Interaction: A comparison of Turn Taking in Japanese and Anglo/American English.Ph.D. dissertation, the University of Oxford.

  • Thompson, S.A. (1998). A discourse explanation for the cross-linguistic differences in the grammar of interrogative and negation.In A. Siewierska and J.J. Song (Eds.), Case, Typology, and Grammar.

  • John Benjamins. Tsui, A. (1992). A functional description of questions.In M. Coulthard (Ed.), Advances in Spoken Discourse Analysis, pp. 89-110.London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E.G. (1993). Varieties of Questions in English Conversation.Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Takagi, T. “Questions” in Argument Sequences in Japanese. Human Studies 22, 397–423 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005419406587

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005419406587

Navigation