Skip to main content
Log in

What is Shared in Joint Action? Issues of Co-representation, Response Conflict, and Agent Identification

  • Joint Action: What is Shared?
  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When sharing a task with another person that requires turn taking, as in doubles games of table tennis, performance on the shared task is similar to performing the whole task alone. This has been taken to indicate that humans co-represent their partner’s task share, as if it were their own. Task co-representation allows prediction of the other’s responses when it is the other’s turn, and leads to response conflict in joint interference tasks. However, data from our lab cast doubt on the view that task co-representation and resulting response conflict are the only or even primary source of effects observed in task sharing. Recent findings furthermore suggest another potential source of interference in joint task performance that has been neglected so far: Self-other discrimination and conflict related to agent identification (i.e., determining whether it is “my” or the other’s turn). Based on these findings we propose that participants might not always co-represent what their partner is supposed to do, but instead co-represent that another agent is responsible for part of the task, and when it is his turn. We call this account the actor co-representation account.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Here and in the following we use the term single response go/nogo task to refer to tasks in which participants have only one response “alternative”, and are either required to respond (go) or to withhold the response (nogo), depending on the task rules, and hence the stimulus presented on a given trial. In contrast, we use the term binary choice task when participants respond on each trial by choosing the appropriate response from two response alternatives. Finally, the term binary choice go/nogo task refers to tasks in which participants respond to some stimuli by choosing between two response alternatives assigned to certain stimuli (go), but have to refrain from responding to other stimuli (nogo).

  2. We thank Andrea Philipp for providing the data of this experiment.

  3. We also analyzed the CNV because previous findings suggests that the late part of the CNV also reflect anticipatory motor preparation when preparing to act oneself as well as when expecting another person’s action (e.g., Kilner et al. 2004; Kourtis et al. 2010). However, in our Exp. 3 there was no significant difference between the CNV found in NoGo trials in the joint and individual task settings. Therefore our results cannot distinguish between the CNV portion that is related to the predictable imperative stimulus and the portion related to action prediction.

  4. Note that neutral flankers in the Atmaca et al. (2011) study cannot distinguish between the two accounts either. This is because neutral flankers (that were not assigned to either agent/response) never occurred in the target position, and hence were not associated with “not my turn” or “withhold response”. Such a condition would be interesting because the actor co-representation account predicts that flanker stimuli assigned to the other agent should lead to more conflict/interference than neutral flankers (also assigned to nogo/withhold responses) in the joint, but not the individual condition. In contrast, the task co-representation account would not predict differences between joint and individual settings for neutral flanker trials because neutral letters are not assigned to the other’s response.

References

  • Aiello, J.R., and E.A. Douthitt. 2001. Social facilitation from Triplett to electronic performance monitoring. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, & Practice 5: 163–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D.M., and C.D. Frith. 2006. Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 268–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansorge, U., and P. Wühr. 2004. A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 365–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atmaca, S., N. Sebanz, W. Prinz, and G. Knoblich. 2008. Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect. Social Neuroscience 3(3–4): 410–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atmaca, S., N. Sebanz, and G. Knoblich. (2011). The Joint Flanker Effect: Sharing tasks with Real and Imagined Co-Actors. Experimental Brain Research.

  • Coles, M.G.H., G. Gratton, T.R. Bashore, C.W. Eriksen, and E. Donchin. 1985. A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11: 529–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M.H. 1980. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology 10: 85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M.H. 1983. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44: 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., C.-C. Liang, and E. Lauber. 1994. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 731–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enz, S. 2009. Empathie als mehrdimensionales Konstrukt im Kontext von sozialen Konflikten in Teamsituationen [Empathy as a multi-dimensional construct in the context of social conflicts and of team situations]. Hamburg: Verlag Dr. Kovac.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B.A., and C.W. Eriksen. 1974. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics 16: 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, R.G., and J.W. Gwynne. 1985. Temporal characteristics of noise conditions producing facilitation and interference. Perception and Psychophysics 37: 495–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guagnano, D., E. Rusconi, and C. Umiltà. 2010. Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of a confederate in action coding. Cognition 114: 348–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerin, B. 1993. Social facilitation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Holländer, A., Jung, C., & Prinz, W. (2011). Covert motor activity on NoGo trials in a task sharing paradigm: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Experimental Brain Research. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2688-x

  • Hommel, B. 2000. The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes, ed. S. Monsell and J. Driver, 247–273. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., J. Müsseler, G. Aschersleben, and W. Prinz. 2001. The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 849–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., L.S. Colzato, and W.P.M. van den Wildenberg. 2009. How social are task representations? Psychological Science 20: 794–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J.M., C. Vargas, S. Duval, S.-J. Blakemore, and A. Sirigu. 2004. Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nature Neuroscience 7: 1299–1301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., and A.M. Philipp. 2005. Effects of response selection on the task-repetition benefit in task switching. Memory & Cognition 33: 624–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., T. Hasbroucq, and A. Osman. 1990. Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review 97: 253–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kourtis, D., N. Sebanz, and G. Knoblich. 2010. Favouritism in the motor system: social interaction modulates action simulation. Biology Letters 6: 758–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, M.Y., and R. Chua. 2009. Influence of stimulus-response assignment on the joint-action correspondence effect. Psychological Research 74: 476–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuthold, H., W. Sommer, and R. Ulrich. 2004. Preparing for action: Inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychopysiology 18: 77–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liepelt, R., D. Wenke, R. Fischer, & W. Prinz. 2010. Trial-to-trial sequential dependencies in a social and non-social Simon task. Psychological Research, epub ahead of print, doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0314-3

  • Lu, C.H., and R.W. Proctor. 1995. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2: 174–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W.T., L.A. Valdes, K.M. Terry, and D.S. Gorfein. 1992. Persistence of negative priming. II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18: 993–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, A.M., and W. Prinz. 2010. Evidence for a role of the responding agent in the joint compatibility effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63: 2159–2171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 1997. Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9: 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramnani, N., and R.C. Miall. 2004. A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others. Nature Neuroscience 7: 85–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R.D., and S. Monsell. 1995. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124: 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruys, K.I., and H. Aarts. 2010. When competition merges people’s behavior: Interdependency activates shared action representations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46: 1130–1133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A.F., and J.M. Lamers. 2002. The Erisksen flanker effect revisited. Acta Psychologica 109: 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuch, S., and I. Koch. 2003. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29: 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., and G. Knoblich. 2009. Prediction in joint action: what, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science 1: 353–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., G. Knoblich, and W. Prinz. 2003. Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88: B11–B21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., G. Knoblich, and W. Prinz. 2005. How two share a task: Corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31: 1234–1246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., H. Bekkering, and G. Knoblich. 2006. Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 70–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., D. Rebbechi, G. Knoblich, W. Prinz, and C. Frith. 2007. Is it really my turn? An event-related fMRI study of task sharing. Social Neuroscience 2: 81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J.R. 1990. The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective, ed. R.W. Proctor and T.G. Reeve. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J.R., and A.P. Rudell. 1967. Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology 51: 300–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J.R., J.V. Hinrichs, and J.L. Craft. 1970. Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology 86: 97–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stürmer, B., H. Leuthold, E. Soetens, H. Schröter, and W. Sommer. 2002. Control over location-based response activation in the Simon Task: Behavioral and Electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 1345–1363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tagliabue, M., M. Zorzi, C. Umiltà, and Bassignani. 2000. The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 648–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, C.C., W.J. Kuo, J. Jing, D.L. Hung, and O.J.L. Tzeng. 2006. A common coding framework in self-other interaction. Evidence from a joint action task. Experimental Brain Research 175: 353–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, C.C., W.J. Kuo, D.L. Hung, and O.J.L. Tzeng. 2008. Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(11): 2015–2024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Schie, H.T., R.B. Mars, M.G.H. Coles, and H. Bekkering. 2004. Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation. Nature Neuroscience 7: 549–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vesper, C., S. Butterfill, G. Knoblich, and N. Sebanz. 2010. A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks 23: 998–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T.N. 2009. When 1 + 1 = 1: The unification of independent actors revealed through joint Simon effects in crossed and uncrossed effector conditions. Human Movement Science 28: 726–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T.N., L. Higgins, M. Ray, and D.J. Weeks. 2007. Seeing vs. believing: Is believing sufficient to activate the processes of response co-representation? Human Movement Science 26: 853–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D. 2002. Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science 3(2): 159–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. 1965. Social facilitation. Science 149: 269–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H., J. Zhang, and S. Kornblum. 1999. A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cognitive Psychology 38: 386–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antje Holländer.

Additional information

Silke Atmaca and Antje Holländer contributed equally to this paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wenke, D., Atmaca, S., Holländer, A. et al. What is Shared in Joint Action? Issues of Co-representation, Response Conflict, and Agent Identification. Rev.Phil.Psych. 2, 147–172 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0057-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0057-0

Keywords

Navigation