Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Structure‐Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy.Dedre Gentner - 1983 - Cognitive Science 7 (2):155-170.
    A theory of analogy must describe how the meaning of an analogy is derived from the meanings of its parts. In the structure‐mapping theory, the interpretation rules are characterized as implicit rules for mapping knowledge about a base domain into a target domain. Two important features of the theory are (a) the rules depend only on syntactic properties of the knowledge representation, and not on the specific content of the domains; and (b) the theoretical framework allows analogies to be distinguished (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   534 citations  
  • Features of similarity.Amos Tversky - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (4):327-352.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   601 citations  
  • Absolute Identification by Relative Judgment.Neil Stewart, Gordon D. A. Brown & Nick Chater - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (4):881-911.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • The development of features in object concepts.Philippe G. Schyns, Robert L. Goldstone & Jean-Pierre Thibaut - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (1):1-17.
    According to one productive and influential approach to cognition, categorization, object recognition, and higher level cognitive processes operate on a set of fixed features, which are the output of lower level perceptual processes. In many situations, however, it is the higher level cognitive process being executed that influences the lower level features that are created. Rather than viewing the repertoire of features as being fixed by low-level processes, we present a theory in which people create features to subserve the representation (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  • Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds.Derek C. Penn, Keith J. Holyoak & Daniel J. Povinelli - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2):109-130.
    Over the last quarter century, the dominant tendency in comparative cognitive psychology has been to emphasize the similarities between human and nonhuman minds and to downplay the differences as (Darwin 1871). In the present target article, we argue that Darwin was mistaken: the profound biological continuity between human and nonhuman animals masks an equally profound discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. To wit, there is a significant discontinuity in the degree to which human and nonhuman animals are able to approximate (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   227 citations  
  • Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation.G. Murphy - 1997 - Cognition 62 (1):99-108.
  • On metaphoric representation.Gregory L. Murphy - 1996 - Cognition 60 (2):173-204.
  • SUSTAIN: A Network Model of Category Learning.Bradley C. Love, Douglas L. Medin & Todd M. Gureckis - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):309-332.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning.John K. Kruschke - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):22-44.
  • Varieties of sameness: the impact of relational complexity on perceptual comparisons.James K. Kroger, Keith J. Holyoak & John E. Hummel - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (3):335-358.
    The fundamental relations that underlie cognitive comparisons—“same” and “different”—can be defined at multiple levels of abstraction, which vary in relational complexity. We compared response times to decide whether or not two sequentially‐presented patterns, each composed of two pairs of colored squares, were the same at three levels of abstraction: perceptual, relational, and system (higher order relations). For both 150 ms and 5 s inter‐stimulus intervals (ISIs), both with and without a masking stimulus, decision time increased with level of abstraction. Sameness (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology.Graeme S. Halford, William H. Wilson & Steven Phillips - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (6):803-831.
    Working memory limits are best defined in terms of the complexity of the relations that can be processed in parallel. Complexity is defined as the number of related dimensions or sources of variation. A unary relation has one argument and one source of variation; its argument can be instantiated in only one way at a time. A binary relation has two arguments, two sources of variation, and two instantiations, and so on. Dimensionality is related to the number of chunks, because (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Similarity as transformation.Ulrike Hahn, Nick Chater & Lucy B. Richardson - 2003 - Cognition 87 (1):1-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Context effects and the validity of loudness scales.W. R. Garner - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (3):218.
  • The structure-mapping engine: Algorithm and examples.Brian Falkenhainer, Kenneth D. Forbus & Dedre Gentner - 1989 - Artificial Intelligence 41 (1):1-63.
  • A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts.Leonidas A. A. Doumas, John E. Hummel & Catherine M. Sandhofer - 2008 - Psychological Review 115 (1):1-43.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  • Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Michelene T. H. Chi, Paul J. Feltovich & Robert Glaser - 1981 - Cognitive Science 5 (2):121-52.
    The representation of physics problems in relation to the organization of physics knowledge is investigated in experts and novices. Four experiments examine the existence of problem categories as a basis for representation; differences in the categories used by experts and novices; differences in the knowledge associated with the categories; and features in the problems that contribute to problem categorization and representation. Results from sorting tasks and protocols reveal that experts and novices begin their problem representations with specifiably different problem categories, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   231 citations  
  • Time in the mind: Using space to think about time.Daniel Casasanto & Lera Boroditsky - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):579-593.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • The Career of Metaphor.Brian F. Bowdle & Dedre Gentner - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (1):193-216.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • Similarity involving attributes and relations: Judgments of similarity and difference are not inverses.D. L. Medin, R. L. Goldstone & D. Gentner - 1990 - Psychological Science 1:64-69.
  • Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories.Eleanor Rosch & Carolyn B. Mervis - 1975 - Cognitive Psychology 7 (4):573--605.
    Six experiments explored the hypothesis that the members of categories which are considered most prototypical are those with most attributes in common with other members of the category and least attributes in common with other categories. In probabilistic terms, the hypothesis is that prototypicality is a function of the total cue validity of the attributes of items. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects listed attributes for members of semantic categories which had been previously rated for degree of prototypicality. High positive (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   392 citations  
  • Alcove-an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning.Jk Kruschke & Rm Nosofsky - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (6):475-475.