Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Change blindness.Daniel J. Simons & Daniel T. Levin - 1997 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1 (1):241-82.
  • Vision: Variations on Some Berkeleian Themes.Robert Schwartz & David Marr - 1985 - Philosophical Review 94 (3):411.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   749 citations  
  • Preemption effects in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping.Ronald A. Rensink & James T. Enns - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (1):101-130.
    Experiments are presented showing that visual search for Mueller-Lyer (ML) stimuli is based on complete configurations, rather than component segments. Segments easily detected in isolation were difficult to detect when embedded in a configuration, indicating preemption by low-level groups. This preemption—which caused stimulus components to become inaccessible to rapid search—was an all-or-nothing effect, and so could serve as a powerful test of grouping. It is shown that these effects are unlikely to be due to blurring by simple spatial filters at (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Consciousness Explained.Daniel C. Dennett - 1991 - Penguin Books.
    Little, Brown, 1992 Review by Glenn Branch on Jul 5th 1999 Volume: 3, Number: 27.
  • Consciousness Explained.Daniel C. Dennett - 1993 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 53 (4):905-910.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1369 citations  
  • Vision.David Marr - 1982 - W. H. Freeman.
  • Early completion of occluded objects.Ronald A. Rensink & James T. Enns - 1998 - Vision Research 38:2489-2505.
    We show that early vision can use monocular cues to rapidly complete partially-occluded objects. Visual search for easily detected fragments becomes difficult when the completed shape is similar to others in the display; conversely, search for fragments that are difficult to detect becomes easy when the completed shape is distinctive. Results indicate that completion occurs via the occlusion-triggered removal of occlusion edges and linking of associated regions. We fail to find evidence for a visible filling-in of contours or surfaces, but (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • The Rapid Recovery of Three-Dimensional Structure from Line Drawings.Ronald A. Rensink - 1992 - Dissertation, University of British Columbia
    A computational theory is developed that explains how line drawings of polyhedral objects can be interpreted rapidly and in parallel at early levels of human vision. The key idea is that a time-limited process can correctly recover much of the three-dimensional structure of these objects when split into concurrent streams, each concerned with a single aspect of scene structure.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades.John A. Grimes - 1996 - In Kathleen Akins (ed.), Perception. Oxford University Press.
  • The dynamic representation of scenes.Ronald A. Rensink - 2000 - Visual Cognition 7 (1/2/3):17-42.
    One of the more powerful impressions created by vision is that of a coherent, richly-detailed world where everything is present simultaneously. Indeed, this impression is so compelling that we tend to ascribe these properties not only to the external world, but to our internal representations as well. But results from several recent experiments argue against this latter ascription. For example, changes in images of real-world scenes often go unnoticed when made during a saccade, flicker, blink, or movie cut. This "change (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes.Ronald A. Rensink, J. Kevin O'Regan & James J. Clark - 1997 - Psychological Science 8:368-373.
    When looking at a scene, observers feel that they see its entire structure in great detail and can immediately notice any changes in it. However, when brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: identification of changes becomes extremely difficult, even when changes are large and made repeatedly. Identification is much faster when a verbal cue is provided, showing that poor visibility is not the cause of (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   194 citations  
  • Failure to detect displacements of the visual world during saccadic eye movements.Bruce Bridgeman, David Hendry & L. Stark - 1975 - Vision Research 15:719-22.
  • Information integration across saccadic eye movements.D. E. Irwin - 1991 - Cognitive Psychology 23:420-56.
  • The information available in visual presentations.George Sperling - 1960 - Psychological Monographs 74:1-29.
  • Change blindness blindness: The metacognitive error of overestimating change-detection ability.Daniel T. Levin, Nausheen Momen, Sarah B. Drivdahl & Daniel J. Simons - 2000 - Visual Cognition 7 (1):397-412.
  • The fine structure of psychological time.J. M. Stroud - 1957 - In H. Quastler (ed.), Information Theory in Psychology: Problems and Methods. Free Press.
  • The fine structure of psychological time.J. M. Stroud - 1967 - Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 138:623-631.
  • Familiarity and visual change detection.Harold Pashler - 1988 - Perception and Psychophysics 41:191-201.
  • Is visual information integrated across successive fixations in reading?G. W. McConkie & D. Zola - 1979 - Perception and Psychophysics 25:221-24.