Results for 'Cognitive spatialization'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking.Barbara Tversky & Bridgette Martin Hard - 2009 - Cognition 110 (1):124-129.
    Although people can take spatial perspectives different from their own, it is widely assumed that egocentric perspectives are natural and have primacy. Two studies asked respondents to describe the spatial relations between two objects on a table in photographed scenes; in some versions, a person sitting behind the objects was either looking at or reaching for one of the objects. The mere presence of another person in a position to act on the objects induced a good proportion of respondents to (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  2.  41
    Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures.Daniel B. M. Haun, Christian J. Rapold, Gabriele Janzen & Stephen C. Levinson - 2011 - Cognition 119 (1):70-80.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  3.  70
    Embodied Spatial Cognition.J. Gregory Trafton & Anthony M. Harrison - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (4):686-706.
    We present a spatial system called Specialized Egocentrically Coordinated Spaces embedded in an embodied cognitive architecture (ACT-R Embodied). We show how the spatial system works by modeling two different developmental findings: gaze-following and Level 1 perspective taking. The gaze-following model is based on an experiment by Corkum and Moore (1998), whereas the Level 1 visual perspective-taking model is based on an experiment by Moll and Tomasello (2006). The models run on an embodied robotic system.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  4.  31
    Spatial Semantics, Cognition, and Their Interaction: A Comparative Study of Spatial Categorization in English and Korean.Hongoak Yun & Soonja Choi - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (6):1736-1776.
    This study has two goals. First, we present much‐needed empirical linguistic data and systematic analyses on the spatial semantic systems in English and Korean, two languages that have been extensively compared to date in the debate on spatial language and spatial cognition. We conduct our linguistic investigation comprehensively, encompassing the domains of tight‐ and loose‐fit as well as containment and support relations. The current analysis reveals both cross‐linguistic commonalities and differences: From a common set of spatial features, each language highlights (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  5.  34
    Spatial Subsystem of Moral Metaphors: A Cognitive Semantic Study.Ning Yu, Tianfang Wang & Yingliang He - 2016 - Metaphor and Symbol 31 (4):195-211.
    Cognitive semantic studies have shown that our conceptualization of morality is at least partially metaphorical and that our moral cognition is grounded in some fundamental contrastive categories of our embodied experience in the physical environment. It is argued that our moral cognition is built on a moral metaphor system. Within the framework of conceptual metaphor theory, this study aims to examine the spatial subsystem of moral metaphors in English. We set out with five pairs of moral metaphors that involve (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  6.  30
    Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability.Marcel A. Just & Patricia A. Carpenter - 1985 - Psychological Review 92 (2):137-172.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   105 citations  
  7. Cognitive processing of spatial relations in Euclidean diagrams.Yacin Hamami, Milan N. A. van der Kuil, Ineke J. M. van der Ham & John Mumma - 2020 - Acta Psychologica 205:1--10.
    The cognitive processing of spatial relations in Euclidean diagrams is central to the diagram-based geometric practice of Euclid's Elements. In this study, we investigate this processing through two dichotomies among spatial relations—metric vs topological and exact vs co-exact—introduced by Manders in his seminal epistemological analysis of Euclid's geometric practice. To this end, we carried out a two-part experiment where participants were asked to judge spatial relations in Euclidean diagrams in a visual half field task design. In the first part, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  93
    Spatial Cognition Through the Keyhole: How Studying a Real-World Domain Can Inform Basic Science—and Vice Versa.Madeleine Keehner - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (4):632-647.
    This paper discusses spatial cognition in the domain of minimally invasive surgery. It draws on studies from this domain to shed light on a range of spatial cognitive processes and to consider individual differences in performance. In relation to modeling, the aim is to identify potential opportunities for characterizing the complex interplay between perception, action, and cognition, and to consider how theoretical models of the relevant processes might prove valuable for addressing applied questions about surgical performance and training.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  4
    Cognitive Workload of Tugboat Captains in Realistic Scenarios: Adaptive Spatial Filtering for Transfer Between Conditions.Daniel Miklody & Benjamin Blankertz - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:818770.
    Changing and often class-dependent non-stationarities of signals are a big challenge in the transfer of common findings in cognitive workload estimation using Electroencephalography (EEG) from laboratory experiments to realistic scenarios or other experiments. Additionally, it often remains an open question whether actual cognitive workload reflected by brain signals was the main contribution to the estimation or discriminative and class-dependent muscle and eye activity, which can be secondary effects of changing workload levels. Within this study, we investigated a novel (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Spatial representation, magnitude and the two stems of cognition.Thomas Land - 2014 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 44 (5-6):524-550.
    The aim of this paper is to show that attention to Kant's philosophy of mathematics sheds light on the doctrine that there are two stems of the cognitive capacity, which are distinct, but equally necessary for cognition. Specifically, I argue for the following four claims: The distinctive structure of outer sensible intuitions must be understood in terms of the concept of magnitude. The act of sensibly representing a magnitude involves a special act of spontaneity Kant ascribes to a capacity (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  11.  14
    Spatial cognitive maps in animals: New hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms.Bruno Poucet - 1993 - Psychological Review 100 (2):163-182.
  12.  19
    The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect.Hans-Otto Karnath, David Milner & Giuseppe Vallar (eds.) - 2002 - Oxford University Press.
    Spatial neglect is a disorder of space-related behaviour. It is characterized by failure to explore the side of space contralateral to a brain lesion, or to react or respond to stimuli or subjects located on this side. Research on spatial neglect and related disorders has developed rapidly inrecent years. These advances have been made as a result of neuropsychological studies of patients with brain damage, behavioural studies of animal models, as well as through functional neurophysiological experiments and functional neuroimaging.The (...) and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect provides an overview of this wide-ranging field of scientific endeavour, providing a cohesive synthesis of the most recent observations and results. As well as being a fascinating clinical phenomenon, the study of spatial neglect helps us tounderstand normal mechanisms of directing and maintaining spatial attention and is relevant to the contemporary search for the cerebral correlates of conscious experience, voluntary action and the nature of personal identity itself.The book is divided into seven sections covering the anatomical and neurophysiological bases of the disorder, frameworks of neglect, perceptual and motor factors, the relation to attention, the cognitive processes involved, and strategies for rehabilitation.Chapters have been written by a team of the leading international experts in this field.This will be essential reading for neuropsychologists, neurologists, neurophysiologists, cognitive neuroscientists and psychologists. (shrink)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  9
    The Cognitive Science of Visual‐Spatial Displays: Implications for Design.Mary Hegarty - 2011 - Cognitive Science.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  14.  34
    Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input.Dedre Gentner, Asli Özyürek, Özge Gürcanli & Susan Goldin-Meadow - 2013 - Cognition 127 (3):318-330.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  15.  15
    Spatial Categorization and its Relationship with Cognitive Development: A Pilot Study in Chilean Spanish.Carla Rimassa & Sabela Fernández-Silva - 2014 - Alpha (Osorno) 38:137-154.
    En este artículo presentamos un estudio piloto en que se explora la relación entre la conceptualización del espacio y el desarrollo cognitivo. Para ello se pidió a ocho sujetos de cuatro rangos etarios describir la ubicación espacial de los elementos de una fotografía, a fin de identificar los aspectos que diferencian las expresiones referentes a la ubicación espacial entre rangos etarios. Se observa que, en nuestra muestra, la conceptualización del espacio se complejiza conforme avanza la edad, con diferencias en cinco (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  74
    “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.
    Fundamental to spatial knowledge in all species are the representations underlying object recognition, object search, and navigation through space. But what sets humans apart from other species is our ability to express spatial experience through language. This target article explores the language ofobjectsandplaces, asking what geometric properties are preserved in the representations underlying object nouns and spatial prepositions in English. Evidence from these two aspects of language suggests there are significant differences in the geometric richness with which objects and places (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   100 citations  
  17.  3
    Cognitive Workload and Workload Transitions Elicit Curvilinear Hemodynamics During Spatial Working Memory.Ryan McKendrick & Amanda Harwood - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  18.  36
    Spatial symbol systems and spatial cognition: A computer science perspective on perception-based symbol processing.Christian Freksa, Thomas Barkowsky & Alexander Klippel - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):616-617.
    People often solve spatially presented cognitive problems more easily than their nonspatial counterparts. We explain this phenomenon by characterizing space as an inter-modality that provides common structure to different specific perceptual modalities. The usefulness of spatial structure for knowledge processing on different levels of granularity and for interaction between internal and external processes is described. Map representations are discussed as examples in which the usefulness of spatially organized symbols is particularly evident. External representations and processes can enhance internal representations (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19. Spatial cognition: Embodied and situated.Barbara Tversky - 2009 - In Murat Aydede & P. Robbins (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 201--217.
  20.  8
    Spatial But Not Oculomotor Information Biases Perceptual Memory: Evidence From Face Perception and Cognitive Modeling.Andrea L. Wantz, Janek S. Lobmaier, Fred W. Mast & Walter Senn - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (6):1533-1554.
    Recent research put forward the hypothesis that eye movements are integrated in memory representations and are reactivated when later recalled. However, “looking back to nothing” during recall might be a consequence of spatial memory retrieval. Here, we aimed at distinguishing between the effect of spatial and oculomotor information on perceptual memory. Participants’ task was to judge whether a morph looked rather like the first or second previously presented face. Crucially, faces and morphs were presented in a way that the morph (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21. Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder.Y. Rossetti & G. Rode - 2002 - In Hans-Otto Karnath, David Milner & Giuseppe Vallar (eds.), The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect. Oxford University Press. pp. 375--396.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  22. Cognitive resources and the acquisition of spatial knowledge.Gl Allen - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (6):457-457.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  15
    Remembering spatial cognition as a hippocampal functional component.Verner P. Bingman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):473-474.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  24.  47
    Spatial integration in perception and cognition: An empirical approach to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.Yue Chen - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (1):86-87.
    Evidence for a dysfunction in cognitive coordination in schizophrenia is emerging, but it is not specific enough to prove (or disprove) this long-standing hypothesis. Many aspects of the external world are spatially mapped in the brain. A comprehensive internal representation relies on integration of information across space. Focus on spatial integration in the perceptual and cognitive processes will generate empirical data that shed light on the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  26
    Studying Spatial Conceptualization across Cultures: Anthropology and Cognitive Science.Stephen C. Levinson - 1998 - Ethos: Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology 26 (1):7-24.
  26. Cognition in dyschiria: Edoardo bisiach's theory of spatial disorders and consciousness.Anna Berti - 2004 - Cortex 40 (2):275-80.
  27.  94
    Speech and Gesture in Spatial Language and Cognition Among the Yucatec Mayas.Olivier Le Guen - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (5):905-938.
    In previous analyses of the influence of language on cognition, speech has been the main channel examined. In studies conducted among Yucatec Mayas, efforts to determine the preferred frame of reference in use in this community have failed to reach an agreement (Bohnemeyer & Stolz, 2006; Levinson, 2003 vs. Le Guen, 2006, 2009). This paper argues for a multimodal analysis of language that encompasses gesture as well as speech, and shows that the preferred frame of reference in Yucatec Maya is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  28.  2
    Cognitive exploration drives engagement and re-engagement with imaginary worlds, but not spatial exploration as predicted by evolutionary theory.Rebecca Dunk & Raymond A. Mar - 2022 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45:e281.
    The empirical evidence for exploration underlying the appeal of imaginary worlds is mostly absent or contradictory. Openness, and the cognitive exploration it represents, provides a better account than the overall drive to explore predicted by evolutionary theory. Furthermore, exploration cannot explain why imaginary worlds foster frequent re-engagement.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  7
    Spatial cognition and the avian hippocampus: Research in domestic chicks.Anastasia Morandi-Raikova & Uwe Mayer - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    In this review, we discuss the functional equivalence of the avian and mammalian hippocampus, based mostly on our own research in domestic chicks, which provide an important developmental model. In birds, like in mammals, the hippocampus plays a central role in processing spatial information. However, the structure of this homolog area shows remarkable differences between birds and mammals. To understand the evolutionary origin of the neural mechanisms for spatial navigation, it is important to test how far theories developed for the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  12
    Asymmetric Spatial Processing Under Cognitive Load.Lien Naert, Mario Bonato & Wim Fias - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  17
    Spatial and social cognition in corvids: an evolutionary approach.Russell P. Balda & Alan C. Kamil - 2002 - In Marc Bekoff, Colin Allen & Gordon M. Burghardt (eds.), The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition. MIT Press. pp. 129--134.
  32.  86
    Relative Contribution of Perception/Cognition and Language on Spatial Categorization.Soonja Choi & Kate Hattrup - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (1):102-129.
    This study investigated the relative contribution of perception/cognition and language-specific semantics in nonverbal categorization of spatial relations. English and Korean speakers completed a video-based similarity judgment task involving containment, support, tight fit, and loose fit. Both perception/cognition and language served as resources for categorization, and allocation between the two depended on the target relation and the features contrasted in the choices. Whereas perceptual/cognitive salience for containment and tight-fit features guided categorization in many contexts, language-specific semantics influenced categorization where the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  33. Spatial cognition.Daniel R. Montello - 2001 - In N. J. Smelser & B. Baltes (eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. pp. 7--14771.
  34.  8
    Non-spatial similarity can bias spatial distances in a cognitive map.Xing Xing & Jeffrey A. Saunders - 2022 - Cognition 229 (C):105251.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  7
    Spatial phenomenology and cognitive linguistics: the case of bodily and perceptual spaces.Johan Blomberg & Martin Thiering - 2016 - Metodo. International Studies in Phenomenology and Philosophy 4 (2):159-212.
  36. Spatial cognition.Nora S. Newcombe - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
  37. 8 Spatial cognition: the mental.David R. Olson & Ellen Bialystok - 1982 - In B. De Gelder (ed.), Knowledge and Representation. Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 121.
  38. Spatial cognition.Carol L. Colby & Carl R. Olson - 1999 - In M. J. Zigmond & F. E. Bloom (eds.), Fundamental Neuroscience. pp. 1363--1383.
  39. Spatial Reasoning in Small-size and Large-size Environments: In Search of Early Prefigurations of Spatial Cognition in Small-size Environments.G. Steiner - 1987 - In B. Inhelder, D. De Caprona & A. Cornu-Wells (eds.), Piaget Today. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 203--216.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  18
    Comparative cognition of spatial representation.Donald M. Wilkie & Robert J. Wilison - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):97-98.
  41.  11
    Spatial and cognitive vision differentiate at low levels, but not in language.Bruce Bridgeman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):240-240.
  42.  19
    Characterizing the Details of Spatial Construction: Cognitive Constraints and Variability.Amy Lynne Shelton, E. Emory Davis, Cathryn S. Cortesa, Jonathan D. Jones, Gregory D. Hager, Sanjeev Khudanpur & Barbara Landau - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (1):e13081.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 1, January 2022.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43. Modeling spatial cognition [special issue](Vol. 3)(No. 4).G. Gunzelmann - forthcoming - Topics in Cognitive Science.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  23
    From Innate Spatial Biases to Enculturated Spatial Cognition: The Case of Spatial Associations in Number and Other Sequences.Koleen McCrink & Maria Dolores de Hevia - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
  45.  26
    Does Environmental Experience Shape Spatial Cognition? Frames of Reference Among Ancash Quechua Speakers.A. Shapero Joshua - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (5):1274-1298.
    Previous studies have shown that language contributes to humans' ability to orient using landmarks and shapes their use of frames of reference for memory. However, the role of environmental experience in shaping spatial cognition has not been investigated. This study addresses such a possibility by examining the use of FoRs in a nonverbal spatial memory task among residents of an Andean community in Peru. Participants consisted of 97 individuals from Ancash Quechua-speaking households who spoke Quechua and/or Spanish and varied considerably (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  46.  78
    Phenomenal and Cognitive Factors in Spatial Perception.Gary Hatfield - 2012 - In Gary Hatfield & Sarah Allred (eds.), Visual Experience: Sensation, Cognition, and Constancy. Oxford University Press. pp. 35.
    This chapter provides an overview of the phenomenology of size perception and the use of instructions to tease apart phenomenal and cognitive aspects. It develops his own recent proposals concerning the geometry of visual space. The chapter proposes that visual space is contracted along the lines of sight. This contraction would explain the apparent convergence of railway tracks, but without invoking a “proximal mode” experience. Parallel railway tracks receding into the distance project converging lines onto the retinas. A true (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  47.  11
    Building a Cognitive Science of Human Variation: Individual Differences in Spatial Navigation.Nora S. Newcombe, Mary Hegarty & David Uttal - 2023 - Topics in Cognitive Science 15 (1):6-14.
    This issue assesses how human spatial navigation differs: within individuals across short‐term variations in mood or stress, and between individuals across variations in age, gender, education, culture, and physical environment.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  39
    Whence and whither in spatial language and spatial cognition?Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):255-265.
  49.  21
    An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time.Antonino Vallesi, Malcolm A. Binns & Tim Shallice - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):501-527.
  50.  69
    Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach–withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance.Jeremy R. Gray - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (3):436.
1 — 50 / 1000