Results for 'Neocortex'

93 found
Order:
  1. Is neocortex essentially multisensory?Asif A. Ghazanfar & Charles E. Schroeder - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (6):278-285.
  2.  5
    The neocortex in memory storage.Herbert P. Killackey - 1990 - In J. McGaugh, Jerry Weinberger & G. Lynch (eds.), Brain Organization and Memory. Guilford Press. pp. 265--70.
  3.  47
    Reptilian cortex and mammalian neocortex early developmental homologies.Miguel Marín-Padilla - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (5):560-561.
    I agree with the view expressed in the target article that the early structural organization of the mammalian neocortex (the primordial neocortical organization) is different from its final one and resembles the more primitive organization of reptilian cortex. During the early development of the neocortex, a distinctly mammalian multilayered pyramidal-cell plate is introduced within a more primitive reptilian-like cortex, establishing simultaneously layer I (marginal zone) above it and layer VII (subplate zone) below it. This multilayered pyramidal-cell plate represents (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  16
    Atypical Amygdala–Neocortex Interaction During Dynamic Facial Expression Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorder.Wataru Sato, Takanori Kochiyama, Shota Uono, Sayaka Yoshimura, Yasutaka Kubota, Reiko Sawada, Morimitsu Sakihama & Motomi Toichi - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  5.  26
    Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory.James L. McClelland, Bruce L. McNaughton & Randall C. O'Reilly - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (3):419-457.
  6.  13
    Is the Cerebral Neocortex a Uniform Cognitive Architecture?Martin Ebdon - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (3):368-395.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  7. Constructing the neocortex: influence on the pattern of organization in mammals.L. Krubitzer, K. Huffman & Z. Molnár - forthcoming - Brain and Mind: Evolutionary Perspectives.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  8.  14
    Mosaic evolution of the neocortex.Dean Falk & Bruce Dudek - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):701-702.
  9.  28
    Multisensory integration beyond the neocortex.Asif A. Ghazanfar & Charles E. Schroeder - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (6):278-285.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. GABAergic inhibition in the neocortex.K. Krnjevic - 1987 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 8 (4):537-547.
  11.  20
    Elephants have a large neocortex too.Jeheskel Shoshani - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):100-100.
  12.  11
    Representational formats in medial temporal lobe and neocortex also determine subjective memory features.Nikolai Axmacher - 2019 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42.
    Episodic memories are shaped by the representational format of their contents. These formats are not only determined by medial temporal lobe areas, but essentially also by the neocortical regions which these areas control. The representational formats of medial temporal lobe and neocortex are sufficient to determine both, memory contents and subjective memory qualities, without the further need for an attribution system.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  5
    The structural design of the neocortex.M. L. Colonnier - 1966 - In John C. Eccles (ed.), Brain and Conscious Experience. Springer. pp. 1--23.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  49
    Common scaling laws for city highway systems and the mammalian neocortex.Mark A. Changizi & Marc Destefano - 2010 - Complexity 15 (3):NA-NA.
  15.  40
    What is the computational goal of the neocortex.H. B. Barlow - 1994 - In Christof Koch & J. Davis (eds.), Large-Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain. MIT Press. pp. 1--22.
  16. Emotional feelings originate below the neocortex: Toward a neurobiology of the soul.Jaak Panksepp - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):101-103.
    Disregard of primary-process consciousness is endemic in mind science. Most neuroscientists subscribe to ruthless reductionism whereby mental qualities are discarded in preference for neuronal functions. Such ideas often lead to envisioning other animals, and all too often other humans, as unfeeling zombies. Merker correctly highlights how the roots of consciousness exist in ancient neural territories we share, remarkably homologously, with all the other vertebrates. (Published Online May 1 2007).
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  17.  14
    Cross-frequency coupling within and between the human thalamus and neocortex.Thomas FitzGerald - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  18. Neural mechanisms of binding in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from computational models.Daniel M. Cer & O'Reilly & C. Randall - 2006 - In Hubert Zimmer, Axel Mecklinger & Ulman Lindenberger (eds.), Handbook of Binding and Memory: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  60
    Why is brain size so important:Design problems and solutions as neocortex gets biggeror smaller. [REVIEW]Jon H. Kaas - 2000 - Brain and Mind 1 (1):7-23.
    As bridges or brains become bigger or smaller, the changes pose problems of design thatneed to be solved. Larger brains could have larger or more neurons, or both. With largerneurons, it becomes difficult to maintain conduction times over longer axons andelectrical cable properties over longer dendrites. With more neurons, it becomes difficultfor each neuron to maintain its proportion of connections with other neurons. Theseproblems are addressed by making brains more modular, thereby reducing the lengths ofmany connections, and by altering functions. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  20.  94
    Social network size in humans.R. A. Hill & R. I. M. Dunbar - 2003 - Human Nature 14 (1):53-72.
    This paper examines social network size in contemporary Western society based on the exchange of Christmas cards. Maximum network size averaged 153.5 individuals, with a mean network size of 124.9 for those individuals explicitly contacted; these values are remarkably close to the group size of 150 predicted for humans on the basis of the size of their neocortex. Age, household type, and the relationship to the individual influence network structure, although the proportion of kin remained relatively constant at around (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  21.  79
    Precis of principles of brain evolution.F. Striedter Georg - 2006 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29 (1):1-12.
    Brain evolution is a complex weave of species similarities and differences, bound by diverse rules and principles. This book is a detailed examination of these principles, using data from a wide array of vertebrates but minimizing technical details and terminology. It is written for advanced undergraduates, graduate students, and more senior scientists who already know something about “the brain,” but want a deeper understanding of how diverse brains evolved. The book's central theme is that evolutionary changes in absolute brain size (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  22.  54
    Complementary Learning Systems.Randall C. O’Reilly, Rajan Bhattacharyya, Michael D. Howard & Nicholas Ketz - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1229-1248.
    This paper reviews the fate of the central ideas behind the complementary learning systems (CLS) framework as originally articulated in McClelland, McNaughton, and O’Reilly (1995). This framework explains why the brain requires two differentially specialized learning and memory systems, and it nicely specifies their central properties (i.e., the hippocampus as a sparse, pattern-separated system for rapidly learning episodic memories, and the neocortex as a distributed, overlapping system for gradually integrating across episodes to extract latent semantic structure). We review the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  23. What Is Wrong with the No-Report Paradigm and How to Fix It.Ned Block - 2019 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23 (12):1003-1013.
    Is consciousness based in prefrontal circuits involved in cognitive processes like thought, reasoning, and memory or, alternatively, is it based in sensory areas in the back of the neocortex? The no-report paradigm has been crucial to this debate because it aims to separate the neural basis of the cognitive processes underlying post-perceptual decision and report from the neural basis of conscious perception itself. However, the no-report paradigm is problematic because, even in the absence of report, subjects might engage in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  24.  44
    Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior: A critique of the arousal theory and a new synthesis.C. H. Vanderwolf & T. E. Robinson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):459-476.
    It is traditionally believed that cerebral activation (the presence of low voltage fast electrical activity in the neocortex and rhythmical slow activity in the hippocampus) is correlated with arousal, while deactivation (the presence of large amplitude irregular slow waves or spindles in both the neocortex and the hippocampus) is correlated with sleep or coma. However, since there are many exceptions, these generalizations have only limited validity. Activated patterns occur in normal sleep (active or paradoxical sleep) and during states (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   172 citations  
  25. Fish and microchips: on fish pain and multiple realization.Matthias Michel - 2019 - Philosophical Studies 176 (9):2411-2428.
    Opponents to consciousness in fish argue that fish do not feel pain because they do not have a neocortex, which is a necessary condition for feeling pain. A common counter-argument appeals to the multiple realizability of pain: while a neocortex might be necessary for feeling pain in humans, pain might be realized differently in fish. This paper argues, first, that it is impossible to find a criterion allowing us to demarcate between plausible and implausible cases of multiple realization (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  26.  9
    The origin of language: More words needed.Robert L. Solso - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):386-387.
    Dunbar's idea that neocortex size limits the number of relationships beings may be able to maintain is an engaging hypothesis for cognitive psychologists interested in a limited capacity model. It is suggested that the thesis would have been enhanced had the author considered the concept of peers as part of an information processing scheme.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  43
    The Role of the Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Prediction Error and Signaling Surprise.William H. Alexander & Joshua W. Brown - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (1):119-135.
    In the past two decades, reinforcement learning has become a popular framework for understanding brain function. A key component of RL models, prediction error, has been associated with neural signals throughout the brain, including subcortical nuclei, primary sensory cortices, and prefrontal cortex. Depending on the location in which activity is observed, the functional interpretation of prediction error may change: Prediction errors may reflect a discrepancy in the anticipated and actual value of reward, a signal indicating the salience or novelty of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  28. The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto.Steven R. Quartz & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):537-556.
    How do minds emerge from developing brains? According to the representational features of cortex are built from the dynamic interaction between neural growth mechanisms and environmentally derived neural activity. Contrary to popular selectionist models that emphasize regressive mechanisms, the neurobiological evidence suggests that this growth is a progressive increase in the representational properties of cortex. The interaction between the environment and neural growth results in a flexible type of learning: minimizes the need for prespecification in accordance with recent neurobiological evidence (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   103 citations  
  29. The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness.Stephen Grossberg - 1999 - Consciousness and Cognition 8 (1):1-44.
    The processes whereby our brains continue to learn about a changing world in a stable fashion throughout life are proposed to lead to conscious experiences. These processes include the learning of top-down expectations, the matching of these expectations against bottom-up data, the focusing of attention upon the expected clusters of information, and the development of resonant states between bottom-up and top-down processes as they reach an attentive consensus between what is expected and what is there in the outside world. It (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  30.  57
    Thalamic contributions to attention and consciousness.James Newman - 1995 - Consciousness and Cognition 4 (2):172-93.
    A tacit assumption since the 19th Century has been that the neocortex serves as the "seat of consciousness." An unexpected challenge to that assumption arose in 1949 with the discovery that high-frequency EEG activation associated with an alert state requires the intactness of the brainstem reticular formation. This discovery became the impetus for nearly three decades of research on what came to be known as the reticular activating system. By the 1970s, however, methodological and philosophical controversies led to general (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  31.  45
    The death of the cortical column? Patchwork structure and conceptual retirement in neuroscientific practice.Philipp Haueis - 2021 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 85:101-113.
    In 1981, David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel received the Nobel Prize for their research on cortical columns—vertical bands of neurons with similar functional properties. This success led to the view that “cortical column” refers to the basic building block of the mammalian neocortex. Since the 1990s, however, critics questioned this building block picture of “cortical column” and debated whether this concept is useless and should be replaced with successor concepts. This paper inquires which experimental results after 1981 challenged the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  32.  57
    The myth-ritual complex: A biogenetic structural analysis.Eugene G. D'aquili - 1983 - Zygon 18 (3):247-269.
    The structuring and transformation of myth is presented as a function of a number of brain “operators.” Each operator is understood to represent specifically evolved neural tissue primarily of the neocortex of the brain. Mythmaking as well as other cognitive processes is seen as a behavior arising from the evolution and integration of certain parts of the brain. Human ceremonial ritual is likewise understood as the culmination of a long phylogenetic evolutionary process, and a neural model is presented to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  33.  68
    On the Neurophysiology of Consciousness: 1. An Overview.Joseph E. Bogen - 1995 - Consciousness and Cognition 4 (1):52-62.
    How certain neural mechanisms momentarily endow with the subjective awareness percepts and affects represented elsewhere is more likely to be clarified when structures essential to Mc are identified. The loss of C with bilateral thalmic lesions involving the intralaminar nuclei contrasts with retention of C after large cortical ablations depriving C of specific contents. A role of ILN in the perception of primitive sensations is suggested by their afference of directly ascending pathways. A role for ILN in awareness of cortical (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  34.  40
    On the dangers of conflating strong and weak versions of a theory of consciousness.Matthias Michel & Hakwan Lau - 2020 - Philosophy and the Mind Sciences 1 (II).
    Some proponents of the Integrated Information Theory of consciousness profess strong views on the Neural Correlates of Consciousness, namely that large swathes of the neocortex, the cerebellum, at least some sensory cortices, and the so-called limbic system are all not essential for any form of conscious experiences. We argue that this connection is not incidental. Conflation between strong and weak versions of the theory has led these researchers to adopt definitions of NCC that are inconsistent with their own previous (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  35. Massive Modularity and Brain Evolution.Edouard Machery - 2007 - Philosophy of Science 74 (5):825-838.
    Quartz (2002) argues that some recent findings about the evolution of the brain (Finlay & Darlington, 1995) are inconsistent with evolutionary psychologists’ massive modularity hypothesis. In substance, Quartz contends that since the volume of the neocortex evolved in a concerted manner, natural selection did not act on neocortical systems independently of each other, which is a necessary condition for the massive modularity of our cognition to be true. I argue however that Quartz’s argument fails to undermine the massive modularity (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  36.  31
    Demography and cultural complexity.Kim Sterelny - 2020 - Synthese 198 (9):8557-8580.
    This paper begins by calling attention to a puzzling feature of our deep past: an apparent mis-match between morphological evolution in our lineage, including the expansion of our brain and neocortex, and changes in material culture. Three ideas might explain this mis-match. The apparent mis-match is an illusion: change in material culture is indeed driven by biological evolution, but of a kind difficult to identify in the fossil record; the mismatch is caused by the fact that material culture is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  37.  33
    Evolution and ontogeny of neural circuits.Sven O. E. Ebbesson - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):321-331.
    Recent studies on neural pathways in a broad spectrum of vertebrates suggest that, in addition to migration and an increase in the number of certain select neurons, a significant aspect of neural evolution is a “parcellation” (segregation-isolation) process that involves the loss of selected connections by the new aggregates. A similar process occurs during ontogenetic development. These findings suggest that in many neuronal systems axons do not invade unknown territories during evolutionary or ontogenetic development but follow in their ancestors' paths (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  38.  63
    Hierarchical minds and the perception/cognition distinction.Daniel Williams - 2023 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 66 (2):275-297.
    Recent research in cognitive and computational neuroscience portrays the neocortex as a hierarchically structured prediction machine. Several theorists have drawn on this research to challenge the traditional distinction between perception and cognition – specifically, to challenge the very idea that perception and cognition constitute useful kinds from the perspective of cognitive neuroscience. In place of this traditional taxonomy, such theorists advocate a unified inferential hierarchy subject to substantial bi-directional message passing. I outline the nature of this challenge and then (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  39.  43
    Putting the puzzle together: Toward a general theory of the neural correlates of consciousness.J. B. Newman - 1997 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 4 (1):47-66.
    Part I of this two-part paper provided a broad overview of clinical and experimental findings bearing on the neural correlates of conscious processes. It was argued that several neurocognitive models related to: orienting to the outer world, dream sleep, and the integration of sensory-motor representations, converge upon a core ‘conscious system’, dubbed the extended reticular-thalamic activating system . The functions of the ERTAS, which shares extensive projections with the cerebral cortex, are mostly ‘implicit’, in contrast to the explicit representation of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  40.  18
    Cybernetic Determinants in the Evolution of Brain and Culture.Nikolai Eberhardt - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (1):31-39.
    Within a physicalist-mechanistic worldview, in which we cannot be more than intelligent, self-reproducing biomachines or biobots, fundamentals of a new approach to the science of human self-explanation are outlined. Some a priori logical necessities, or determinants, of any biobot’s control system design are recognized. Evolution had to satisfy them, but neuroscience and cognitive science so far do not clearly see these basics. It is concluded that the old part of the brain still contains the genetically fixed drives, responses, and the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41. Ansiedad y estrés en el parto y depresión postparto.Lourdes Laínez - 2011 - Critica: La Reflexion Calmada Desenreda Nudos 61 (974):76-79.
    El paro es un proceso fisiológico e involuntario, que está dirigido por el llamado "cerebro primitivo o arcaico", (que tenemos en común con el resto de los mamíferos), y que segrega las hormonas para que las contracciones uterinas sean eficaces en cada etapa. Dichas hormonas sólo se secretan si la actividad del neocórtex, o "cerebro nuevo", (racional, científico y exclusivamente humano), está reducida, ya que esta actividad inhibe los proceso instintivos e involuntarios que dirige dicho cerebro arcaico.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  43
    Brains evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions.Wendy K. Wilkins & Jennie Wakefield - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):161-182.
    This target article presents a plausible evolutionary scenario for the emergence of the neural preconditions for language in the hominid lineage. In pleistocene primate lineages there was a paired evolutionary expansion of frontal and parietal neocortex (through certain well-documented adaptive changes associated with manipulative behaviors) resulting, in ancestral hominids, in an incipient Broca's region and in a configurationally unique junction of the parietal, occipital, and temporal lobes of the brain (the POT). On our view, the development of the POT (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  43.  33
    Consciousness, the brain and what matters.Grant Gillett - 1990 - Bioethics 4 (3):181–198.
    Grant Gillett argues that it is consciousness which makes a human or other being the 'locus of ethical value'. Since cortical functioning is, in Gillett's view, necessary for conscious activity, an individual whose neocortex is permanently non-functional is no longer a locus of ethical value and cannot be benefited or harmed in a morally relevant sense. This means that there is no obligation to continue treating those who have suffered neocortical death.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  44. The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory.Francesco P. Battaglia, Karim Benchenane, Anton Sirota, Cyriel M. A. Pennartz & Sidney I. Wiener - 2011 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (7):310-318.
    A complex brain network, centered on the hippocampus, supports episodic memories throughout their lifetimes. Classically, upon memory encoding during active behavior, hippocampal activity is dominated by theta oscillations (6-10Hz). During inactivity, hippocampal neurons burst synchronously, constituting sharp waves, which can propagate to other structures, theoretically supporting memory consolidation. This 'two-stage' model has been updated by new data from high-density electrophysiological recordings in animals that shed light on how information is encoded and exchanged between hippocampus, neocortex and subcortical structures such (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  45.  35
    Consciousness and brain function.Grant R. Gillett - 1988 - Philosophical Psychology 1 (3):325-39.
    Abstract The language of consciousness and that of brain function seem vastly different and incommensurable ways of approaching human mental life. If we look at what we mean by consciousness we find that it has a great deal to do with the sensitivity and responsiveness shown by a subject toward things that happen. Philosophically, we can understnd ascriptions of consciousness best by looking at the conditions which make it true for thinkers who share the concept to say that one of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  46. Did consciousness cause the cambrian evolutionary explosion?Stuart R. Hameroff - 1998 - In Stuart R. Hameroff, Alfred W. Kaszniak & A. C. Scott (eds.), Toward a Science of Consciousness II. MIT Press. pp. 2--421.
    When and where did consciousness emerge in the course of evolution? Did it happen as recently as the past million years, for example concomitant with language or tool making in humans or primates? Or did consciousness arrive somewhat earlier, with the advent of mammalian neocortex 200 million years ago (Eccles, 1992)? At the other extreme, is primitive consciousness a property of even simple unicellular organisms of several billion years ago (e.g. as suggested by Margulis and Sagan, 1995)? Or did (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  47.  56
    Neuromaturation of the human fetus.Michael J. Flower - 1985 - Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 10 (3):237-252.
    The fetal human possesses an active central nervous system from at least the eighth week of development. Until mid-gestation the most significant center of activity is the brainstem. By the end of the first trimester, it appears that the brainstem could be acting as a rudimentary modulator of sensory information and motor activity. What importance ought to be attached to such regulatory activity is uncertain. Some argue that it represents a level of integrated activity sufficient to bolster an argument for (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  48.  40
    Composition and replay of mnemonic sequences: The contributions of REM and slow-wave sleep to episodic memory.Sen Cheng & Markus Werning - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (6):610-611.
    We propose that rapid eye movement (REM) and slow-wave sleep contribute differently to the formation of episodic memories. REM sleep is important for building up invariant object representations that eventually recur to gamma-band oscillations in the neocortex. In contrast, slow-wave sleep is more directly involved in the consolidation of episodic memories through replay of sequential neural activity in hippocampal place cells.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49. Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain.Axel Cleeremans - manuscript
    The goal of computational cognitive neuroscience is to understand how the brain embodies the mind by using biologically based computational models comprised of networks of neuronlike units. This text, based on a course taught by Randall O'Reilly and Yuko Munakata over the past several years, provides an in-depth introduction to the main ideas in the field. The neural units in the simulations use equations based directly on the ion channels that govern the behavior of real neurons and the neural networks (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50. "The Choreography of the Soul": Recursive Patterns in Psychology, Political Anthropology and Cosmology.Edward D'angelo - 1988 - Dissertation, State University of New York at Stony Brook
    The component structures of two distinct neuropsychological systems are described. "System-Y" depends upon "system-X" which, on the other hand, can operate independently of system-Y. System-X provides a matrix upon which system-Y must operate, and, system-Y is transformed by the operations of system-X. In addition these neuropsychological structures reverberate in political history and in the cosmos. The most fundamental structure in the soul, in society, and in the cosmos, has the form of a conical spiral. It can be described mathematically as (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 93