Results for 'PAST-TENSE'

996 found
Order:
  1.  65
    The Past-Tense Debate The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular forms can (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  2.  25
    The past-tense debate.S. Pinker & M. T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular forms can (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  3.  35
    Past Tense and Past Times in Subjunctive Conditionals.John Mackay - 2017 - Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 98 (S1):520-535.
    Some theories of conditionals maintain that the difference between indicative and subjunctive conditionals involves the standard temporal interpretation of past tense. I provide an argument against such theories. The argument is based on the claim that these views do not correctly predict the correspondence between tense marking and temporal interpretation in certain conditionals.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  13
    Simple Past Tense Markers in Turkish and Some American Indian Languages in Terms of Evidentiality.Demi̇rci̇ Kerim - 2010 - Journal of Turkish Studies 5:281-293.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  21
    Past Tenses.J. G. F. Powell - 1994 - The Classical Review 44 (01):92-.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  35
    Past tense route priming.Emily R. Cohen-Shikora & David A. Balota - 2013 - Cognition 126 (3):397-404.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  7.  26
    The Past Tense View of Counterfactuals Revisited.Moritz Schulz - 2019 - Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy 6.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  14
    The English past tense: Analogy redux.Steve Chandler - 2010 - Cognitive Linguistics 21 (3):371-417.
    The debate over how best to characterize inflectional morphology has been couched largely in terms of the “dual-mechanism” approach described in Pinker (Words and rules: the ingredients of language, Basic Books, 1999) versus “single-mechanism” connectionist approaches derived from Rumelhart and McClelland (On learning past tenses of English verbs, MIT, 1986). There are, however, other single-mechanism approaches. The exemplar-based or analogical models of Daelemans et al. (TimBL: Tilburg Memory-Based Learner, version 4.3 reference guide, ILK, 2002) and Skousen (Analogical modeling of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9. Past tense learning.Amit Almor - 2002 - In M. Arbib (ed.), The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks. MIT Press. pp. 848--851.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. English past tense inflection: regular vs. irregular or easy vs. hard.G. Westermann, V. Kovic & N. Ruh - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 739--744.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  29
    The Past Tenses of the Subjunctive in Expressions of Wish.E. A. Sonnenschein - 1911 - The Classical Review 25 (08):244-246.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Past tense, logic and pedagogy.L. Tasmowskideryck - 1985 - Revue Internationale de Philosophie 39 (155):375-387.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  58
    Universal and Past-Tense Prescriptions: A Reply to Mr. Ibberson.R. M. Hake - 1979 - Analysis 39 (4):161 - 165.
    Properly universal prescriptions (necessary in analysis of value-judgments) entail past-tense imperatives. does the unusability of the latter rule out the former? no, because there are many usable rules which entail past-tense imperatives. else we could not point to past breaches when teaching the rule, which remains the same throughout the teaching process, or punish for past breaches of the same rule which is still in force. similar problem about imperatives in other than the second (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  14.  10
    Memory, Expression, and PastTense Self‐Knowledge.William Child - 2007 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 73 (1):54-76.
    How should we understand our capacity to remember our past intentional states? And what can we leam from Wittgenstein's treatment of this topic? Three questions are considered. First, what is the relation between our past attitudes and our present beliefs about them? Realism about past attitudes is defended. Second, how should we understand Wittgenstein's view that self‐ascriptions of past attitudes are a kind of “response” and that the “language‐game” of reporting past attitudes is “the primary (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  15. Memory, expression, and past-tense self-knowledge.William Child - 2006 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 73 (1):54–76.
    How should we understand our capacity to remember our past intentional states? And what can we learn from Wittgenstein's treatment of this topic? Three questions are considered. First, what is the relation between our past attitudes and our present beliefs about them? Realism about past attitudes is defended. Second, how should we understand Wittgenstein's view that self-ascriptions of past attitudes are a kind of "response" and that the "language-game" of reporting past attitudes is "the primary (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  16. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: a computational/experimental study.Adam Albright & Bruce Hayes - 2003 - Cognition 90 (2):119-161.
    Are morphological patterns learned in the form of rules? Some models deny this, attributing all morphology to analogical mechanisms. The dual mechanism model (Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1998). On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73-193) posits that speakers do internalize rules, but that these rules are few and cover only regular processes; the remaining patterns are attributed to analogy. This article advocates a third approach, which uses multiple stochastic rules (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  17.  52
    Quasiregularity and Its Discontents: The Legacy of the Past Tense Debate.Mark S. Seidenberg & David C. Plaut - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1190-1228.
    Rumelhart and McClelland's chapter about learning the past tense created a degree of controversy extraordinary even in the adversarial culture of modern science. It also stimulated a vast amount of research that advanced the understanding of the past tense, inflectional morphology in English and other languages, the nature of linguistic representations, relations between language and other phenomena such as reading and object recognition, the properties of artificial neural networks, and other topics. We examine the impact of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  18.  32
    Past Tenses Wolfgang Klug: Erzählstruktur ah Kunstform. Studien zur künstlerischen Funktion der Erzähltempora im Lateinischen und im Griechischen. Pp. 206. Heidelberg: Manutius Verlag, 1992. DM 98. [REVIEW]J. G. F. Powell - 1994 - The Classical Review 44 (01):92-94.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  3
    Thinking in the past tense: eight conversations.Alexander Bevilacqua - 2019 - Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Edited by Frederic Clark.
    Ann M. Blair -- Lorraine Daston -- Benjamin Elman -- Anthony Grafton -- Jill Kraye -- Peter N. Miller -- Jean-Louis Quantin -- Quentin Skinner.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  16
    A neuroconstructivist model of past tense development and processing.Gert Westermann & Nicolas Ruh - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (3):649-667.
  21. Understanding the past tense.Christopher Peacocke - 2001 - In Christoph Hoerl & Teresa McCormark (eds.), Time and Memory. Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  22.  61
    The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular forms can (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  23.  37
    Answering the connectionist challenge: a symbolic model of learning the past tenses of English verbs.C. X. Ling & M. Marinov - 1993 - Cognition 49 (3):235-290.
    Supporters of eliminative connectionism have argued for a pattern association-based explanation of language learning and language processing. They deny that explicit rules and symbolic representations play any role in language processing and cognition in general. Their argument is based to a large extent on two artificial neural network (ANN) models that are claimed to be able to learn the past tenses of English verbs (Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986, Parallel distributed processing, Vol. 2, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; MacWhinney & Leinbach, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  24.  8
    Universal and past-tense prescriptions: a reply to Mr. Ibberson.R. M. Hake - 1979 - Analysis 39 (4):161-165.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25. Predicting irregular past tenses.Charles X. Ling - 1994 - In Ashwin Ram & Kurt Eiselt (eds.), Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum. pp. 577--582.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  8
    Knowledge in the Past Tense.Bart Pattyn - 2006 - Ethical Perspectives 13 (2):191-219.
    The traditional concern universities have had with public, universal knowledge seems to be waning, with an ever-greater stress upon privatised knowledge. Nevertheless, this is an old quarrel. Since Plato saw knowledge as in service of society, he scorned the Sophists for commercialising knowledge. For the mediaeval university, which continued and developed certain strands of Plato’s thinking, the privatisation of knowledge was also unthinkable, since all knowledge ultimately belonged to God.The success of the mediaeval university lay in its autonomy, and its (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27. Factual vs. evidential?: The past tense forms of spoken Khalkha Mongolian.Benjamin Brosig - 2018 - In Ad Foolen, Helen de Hoop & Gijs Mulder (eds.), Evidence for evidentiality. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  53
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.Kim Plunkett & Patrick Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  29.  11
    ""Middle Mongolian Past-Tense-" BA" in the" Secret History".John C. Street - 2008 - Journal of the American Oriental Society 128 (3):399-422.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  64
    Reference time and the English past tenses.W. P. M. Meyer-Viol & H. S. Jones - 2011 - Linguistics and Philosophy 34 (3):223-256.
    We offer a formal account of the English past tenses. We see the perfect as having reference time at speech time and the preterite as having reference time at event time. We formalize four constraints on reference time, which we bundle together under the term ‘perspective’. Once these constraints are satisfied at the different reference times of the perfect and preterite, the contrasting functions of these tenses are explained. Thus we can account formally for the ‘definiteness effect’ and the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  47
    Children's Acquisition of the English PastTense: Evidence for a Single‐Route Account From Novel Verb Production Data.Ryan P. Blything, Ben Ambridge & Elena V. M. Lieven - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):621-639.
    This study adjudicates between two opposing accounts of morphological productivity, using English past-tense as its test case. The single-route model posits that both regular and irregular past-tense forms are generated by analogy across stored exemplars in associative memory. In contrast, the dual-route model posits that regular inflection requires use of a formal “add -ed” rule that does not require analogy across regular past-tense forms. Children saw animations of an animal performing a novel action described (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  15
    Interpreting dissociations between regular and irregular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Paul de Mornay Davies & Diane Swick - 2008 - Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 8 (2):178–194.
    Neuropsychological dissociations between regular and irregular English past-tense morphology have been reported using a lexical decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets. We present N400 event-related potential data from healthy participants using the same design. Both regular and irregular past-tense forms primed corresponding present-tense forms, but with a longer duration for irregular verbs. Phonological control conditions suggested that differences in formal overlap between prime and target contribute to, but do (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  12
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.V. Merlin, M. Tataru, F. Valognes, K. Plunkett & P. Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  34.  17
    The acquisition of the English past tense in children and multilayered connectionist networks.Gary F. Marcus - 1995 - Cognition 56 (3):271-279.
    The apparent very close similarity between the learning of the past tense by Adam and the Plunkett and Marchman model is exaggerated by several misleading comparisons--including arbitrary, unexplained changes in how graphs were plotted. The model's development differs from Adam's in three important ways: Children show a U-shaped sequence of development which does not depend on abrupt changes in input; U-shaped development in the simulation occurs only after an abrupt change in training regimen. Children overregularize vowel-change verbs more (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  35. Immunity to error through misidentification and past-tense memory judgements.J. L. Bermudez - 2013 - Analysis 73 (2):211-220.
    Autobiographical memories typically give rise either to memory reports (“I remember going swimming”) or to first person past-tense judgements (“I went swimming”). This article focuses on first person past-tense judgements that are (epistemically) based on autobiographical memories. Some of these judgements have the IEM property of being immune to error through misidentification. This article offers an account of when and why first person past-tense judgements have the IEM property.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  36.  44
    Learning from a connectionist model of the acquisition of the English past tense.Kim Plunkett & Virginia A. Marchman - 1996 - Cognition 61 (3):299-308.
    Comments on G. Marcus' criticisms (see record 1996-24670-001) of K. Plunkett's and V. Marcham's (see record 1994-35650-001) connectionist account of the acquisition of the English past tense (verb morphology). The original model is reviewed. Graphing, overregularization, and other criticisms are addressed (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2000 APA, all rights reserved).
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  37.  17
    The role of Broca's area in regular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Jennifer Yang, Paul de Mornay Davies, Nina Dronkers & Diane Swick - 2011 - Neuropsychologia 49 (1):1–18.
    It has been suggested that damage to anterior regions of the left hemisphere results in a dissociation in the perception and lexical activation of past-tense forms. Specifically, in a lexical-decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets, such patients demonstrate significant priming for irregular verbs (spoke–speak), but, unlike control participants, fail to do so for regular verbs (looked–look). Here, this behavioral dissociation was first confirmed in a group of eleven patients with damage to (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38. The English resultative perfect and its relationship to the experiential perfect and the simple past tense.Anita Mittwoch - 2008 - Linguistics and Philosophy 31 (3):323-351.
    A sentence in the Resultative perfect licenses two inferences: (a) the occurrence of an event (b) the state caused by this event obtains at evaluation time. In this paper I show that this use of the perfect is subject to a large number of distributional restrictions that all serve to highlight the result inference at the expense of the event inference. Nevertheless, only the event inference determines the truth conditions of this use of the perfect, the result inference being a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  14
    When Children's Production Deviates From Observed Input: Modeling the Variable Production of the English Past Tense.Libby Barak, Zara Harmon, Naomi H. Feldman, Jan Edwards & Patrick Shafto - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (8):e13328.
    As children gradually master grammatical rules, they often go through a period of producing form‐meaning associations that were not observed in the input. For example, 2‐ to 3‐year‐old English‐learning children use the bare form of verbs in settings that require obligatory past tense meaning while already starting to produce the grammatical –ed inflection. While many studies have focused on overgeneralization errors, fewer studies have attempted to explain the root of this earlier stage of rule acquisition. In this work, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  31
    Why do children learn to say “Broke”? A model of learning the past tense without feedback.Niels A. Taatgen & John R. Anderson - 2002 - Cognition 86 (2):123-155.
  41. What is the meaning of the present and past tenses?Tim Stowell - manuscript
    What is the meaning of the present and past tenses? The answer to this question depends on what objects these terms refer to. If the question is about the English tense morphemes present and past, we will get one answer; if it is about their Japanese or Russian counterparts, we will get another; and if it is about a semantic categories PRESENT and PAST attributed to the theory of Universal Grammar (UG), we will get still another. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  42. Children's Frequency , Productivity Phonology, in the and English Past Tense : The Role of Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school-aged children (N= 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using on elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero-marking, e.g., sit + sit; vowel-change, e.g., ride -+ rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43. The Past and Future of the Past Tense Debate.Steven Pinker & Michael T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular forms can (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  65
    Rules, representations, and the English past tense.William Marslen-Wilson & Lorraine K. Tyler - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (11):428-435.
  45. Children's productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  46.  31
    The effect of frequency and phonological neighbourhood density on the acquisition of past tense verbs by Finnish children.Minna Kirjavainen, Alexandre Nikolaev & Evan Kidd - 2012 - Cognitive Linguistics 23 (2).
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  47. A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics.Chloe R. Marshall & Heather K. J. van der Lely - 2006 - Cognition 100 (2):302-320.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  48.  23
    Errors of Omission in English‐Speaking Children's Production of Plurals and the Past Tense: The Effects of Frequency, Phonology, and Competition.Danielle E. Matthews & Anna L. Theakston - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (6):1027-1052.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  49. Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology.Mark S. Seidenberg & Maggie Bruck - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (6):522-522.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  50.  27
    The role of meaning in past-tense inflection: Evidence from polysemy and denominal derivation.Shoba Bandi-Rao & Gregory L. Murphy - 2007 - Cognition 104 (1):150-162.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 996