Results for 'Spatial behavior'

999 found
Order:
  1. Spatial behavior, food storing, and the modular mind.Sara J. Shettleworth - 2002 - In Marc Bekoff, Colin Allen & Gordon M. Burghardt (eds.), The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition. MIT Press. pp. 123--128.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  19
    Relationship between performance on the Everyday Spatial Activities Test and on objective measures of spatial behavior in men and women.William W. Beatty & Dee Duncan - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (3):228-230.
  3.  18
    World graphs: A partial model of spatial behavior.Israel Lieblich & Michael A. Arbib - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):651-659.
  4.  33
    Spatial and temporal uncertainty as determinants of vigilance behavior.Jack A. Adams & Lawrence R. Boulter - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (2):127.
  5.  13
    Relating spatial perspective taking to the perception of other's affordances: providing a foundation for predicting the future behavior of others.Sarah H. Creem-Regehr, Kyle T. Gagnon, Michael N. Geuss & Jeanine K. Stefanucci - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  6.  15
    Rationalizable behavior in the Hotelling–Downs model of spatial competition.Joep van Sloun - 2023 - Theory and Decision 95 (2):309-335.
    We consider two scenarios of the Hotelling–Downs model of spatial competition. This setting has typically been explored using pure Nash equilibrium, but this paper uses point rationalizability (Bernheim, Econometrica J Economet Soc 52(4):1007–1028, 1984) instead. Pure Nash equilibrium imposes a correct beliefs assumption, which may rule out perfectly reasonable choices in a game. Point rationalizability does not have this correct beliefs assumption, which makes this solution concept more natural and permissive. The first scenario is the original Hotelling–Downs model with (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Studying temporal and spatial patterns in perceptual behavior: Implications for dynamical structure.Deborah J. Aks - 2009 - In Stephen J. Guastello, Matthijs Koopmans & David Pincus (eds.), Chaos and Complexity in Psychology: The Theory of Nonlinear Dynamical Systems. Cambridge University Press. pp. 132--176.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Sex-dimorphic behavior patterns, maturational timing, and gender differences in spatial ability.D. C. Geary - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (6):499-499.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9.  27
    Changing spaces, changing behaviours: Achaemenid spatial features at the court of Alexander the Great.Stephen Harrison - 2018 - Journal of Ancient History 6 (2):185-214.
    Alexander’s conquest of Persia transformed the way he ruled, with aspects of Achaemenid monarchy becoming prominent. In general, historians have focused on instances of deliberate engagement with Achaemenid practices, leading to the impression that this change resulted from conscious imitation. Here, I nuance this view, arguing that the gradual adoption of aspects of Achaemenid royal space played a pivotal role in transforming Alexander’s monarchy. This approach shifts our focus away from Alexander himself, placing his reign in a wider context, while (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  7
    Influence of Air Quality on Pro-environmental Behavior of Chinese Residents: From the Perspective of Spatial Distance.Guanghua Sheng, Jiatong Dai & Hong Pan - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Although environmental issues have attracted public attention, there are still many people unwilling to make behavioral changes to solve the problem, which makes promoting pro-environmental behavior become an interesting research topic. This study discusses the influence of air quality on the pro-environmental behavior of Chinese residents from the perspective of spatial distance, providing a theoretical basis and practical application for improving pro-environmental behavior. Through three experiments, this study reveals that air pollution within the local spatial (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  11. Reconsidering 'spatial memory' and the Morris water maze.Jacqueline Anne Sullivan - 2010 - Synthese 177 (2):261-283.
    The Morris water maze has been put forward in the philosophy of neuroscience as an example of an experimental arrangement that may be used to delineate the cognitive faculty of spatial memory (e.g., Craver and Darden, Theory and method in the neurosciences, University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, 2001; Craver, Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2007). However, in the experimental and review literature on the water maze throughout the history of its (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  12.  5
    Ecological Location of a Water Source and Spatial Dynamics of Behavior Under Temporally Scheduled Water Deliveries in a Modified Open-Field System: An Integrative Approach.Alejandro León, Varsovia Hernández, Ursula Huerta, Carlos Alberto Hernández-Linares, Porfirio Toledo, Martha Lorena Avendaño Garrido, Esteban Escamilla Navarro & Isiris Guzmán - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    It has been reported in non-contingent schedules that the variety of patterns of behavior is affected by the temporal variation of water deliveries. While temporal variation is accomplished by delivering water at fixed or variable times, spatial variation is usually accomplished by varying the number of dispensers and distance among them. Such criteria do not consider the possible ecological relevance of the location of water dispensers. Nevertheless, it is plausible to suppose that the intersection of the programed contingencies, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  18
    Local Norms and the Theory of Planned Behavior: Understanding the Effects of Spatial Proximity on Recycling Intentions and Self-Reported Behavior.Paola Passafaro, Stefano Livi & Ankica Kosic - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  14.  21
    Time and space in neuronal networks: The effects of spatial organization on network behavior.Stephen P. Womble & Netta Cohen - 2010 - Complexity 16 (2):45-50.
  15. Spatialization and Greater Generosity in the Stochastic Prisoner's Dilemma.Patrick Grim - 1996 - Biosystems 37:3-17.
    The iterated Prisoner’s Dilemma has become the standard model for the evolution of cooperative behavior within a community of egoistic agents, frequently cited for implications in both sociology and biology. Due primarily to the work of Axelrod (1980a, 198Ob, 1984, 1985), a strategy of tit for tat (TFT) has established a reputation as being particularly robust. Nowak and Sigmund (1992) have shown, however, that in a world of stochastic error or imperfect communication, it is not TFT that finally triumphs (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  16.  96
    The Effect of Working Memory Updating Ability on Spatial Insight Problem Solving: Evidence From Behavior and Eye Movement Studies.Qiang Xing, Zheyi Lu & Jing Hu - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  12
    Walking blindfolded unveils unique contributions of behavioural approach and inhibition to lateral spatial bias.Mario Weick, John A. Allen, Milica Vasiljevic & Bo Yao - 2016 - Cognition 147 (C):106-112.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  21
    Spatial navigation, episodic memory, episodic future thinking, and theory of mind in children with autism spectrum disorder: evidence for impairments in mental simulation?Sophie E. Lind, Dermot M. Bowler & Jacob Raber - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:113592.
    This study explored spatial navigation alongside several other cognitive abilities that are thought to share common underlying neurocognitive mechanisms (e.g., the capacity for self-projection, scene construction, or mental simulation), and which we hypothesised may be impaired in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Twenty intellectually high-functioning children with ASD (with a mean age of ~8 years) were compared to 20 sex, age, IQ, and language ability matched typically developing children on a series of tasks to assess spatial navigation, episodic memory, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19.  35
    Exploiting Spatial and Temporal for Point of Interest Recommendation.Jinpeng Chen, Wen Zhang, Pei Zhang, Pinguang Ying, Kun Niu & Ming Zou - 2018 - Complexity 2018:1-16.
    An increasing number of users have been attracted by location-based social networks in recent years. Meanwhile, user-generated content in online LBSNs like spatial, temporal, and social information provides an ever-increasing chance to study the human behavior movement from their spatiotemporal mobility patterns and spawns a large number of location-based applications. For instance, one of such applications is to produce personalized point of interest recommendations that users are interested in. Different from traditional recommendation methods, the recommendations in LBSNs come (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  20.  33
    Spatial Reasoning With External Visualizations: What Matters Is What You See, Not Whether You Interact.Madeleine Keehner, Mary Hegarty, Cheryl Cohen, Peter Khooshabeh & Daniel R. Montello - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (7):1099-1132.
    Three experiments examined the effects of interactive visualizations and spatial abilities on a task requiring participants to infer and draw cross sections of a three‐dimensional (3D) object. The experiments manipulated whether participants could interactively control a virtual 3D visualization of the object while performing the task, and compared participants who were allowed interactive control of the visualization to those who were not allowed control. In Experiment 1, interactivity produced better performance than passive viewing, but the advantage of interactivity disappeared (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  29
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  22. (Re)framing Spatiality as a Socio-cultural Paradigm: Examining the Iranian Housing Culture and Processes.Lakshmi Rajendran, Fariba Molki, Sara Mahdizadeh & Asma Mehan - 2021 - Journal of Architecture and Urbanism 45 (1):95-105.
    With rapid changes in urban living today, peoples’ behavioural patterns and spatial practices undergo a constant process of adaptation and negotiation. Using “house” as a laboratory and everyday life and spatial relations of residents as a framework of analysis, the paper examines the spatial planning concepts in traditional and contemporary Iranian architecture and the associated socio-cultural practices. Discussions are drawn upon from a pilot study conducted in the city of Kerman, to investigate ways in which contemporary housing (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  37
    Behavioural modernity, investigative disintegration & Rubicon expectation.Adrian Currie & Andra Meneganzin - 2022 - Synthese 200 (1):1-28.
    Abstract‘Behavioural modernity’ isn’t what it used to be. Once conceived as an integrated package of traits demarcated by a clear archaeological signal in a specific time and place, it is now disparate, archaeologically equivocal, and temporally and spatially spread. In this paper we trace behavioural modernity’s empirical and theoretical developments over the last three decades, as surprising discoveries in the material record, as well the reappraisal of old evidence, drove increasingly sophisticated demographic, social and cultural models of behavioural modernity. We (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  24.  11
    Spatializing Culture: The Ethnography of Space and Place by Setha Low (review).Carlos J. L. Balsas - 2023 - Environment, Space, Place 15 (1):151-156.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Reviewed by:Spatializing Culture: The Ethnography of Space and Place by Setha LowCarlos J. L. BalsasSpatializing Culture: The Ethnography of Space and Placeby setha low London: Routledge, 2017Spatializing Culture: The Ethnography of Space and Place adds clarity to our understanding of the value of ethnographic scholarship in the study of socio-economic, cultural, and developmental transformations. The book is a thorough review of two established conceptual frames of analysis—the social production (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  20
    Correlating velocity patterns with spatial dynamics in glioma cell migration.Thomas S. Deisboeck, Tim Demuth & Yuri Mansury - 2005 - Acta Biotheoretica 53 (3):181-190.
    Highly malignant neuroepithelial tumors are known for their extensive tissue invasion. Investigating the relationship between their spatial behavior and temporal patterns by employing detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA), we report here that faster glioma cell motility is accompanied by both greater predictability of the cells' migration velocity and concomitantly, more directionality in the cells' migration paths. Implications of this finding for both experimental and clinical cancer research are discussed.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  27
    Neurophysiological and Neuropsychological Aspects of Spatial Neglect.Marc Jeannerod (ed.) - 1987 - Elsevier Science.
    In this volume, three aspects are examined: a) normal subjects, where new findings on spatial behavior are described. b) brain-lesioned subjects, where the ...
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  27.  14
    Modeling Mental Spatial Reasoning About Cardinal Directions.Holger Schultheis, Sven Bertel & Thomas Barkowsky - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (8):1521-1561.
    This article presents research into human mental spatial reasoning with orientation knowledge. In particular, we look at reasoning problems about cardinal directions that possess multiple valid solutions , at human preferences for some of these solutions, and at representational and procedural factors that lead to such preferences. The article presents, first, a discussion of existing, related conceptual and computational approaches; second, results of empirical research into the solution preferences that human reasoners actually have; and, third, a novel computational model (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  19
    Characterizing the Details of Spatial Construction: Cognitive Constraints and Variability.Amy Lynne Shelton, E. Emory Davis, Cathryn S. Cortesa, Jonathan D. Jones, Gregory D. Hager, Sanjeev Khudanpur & Barbara Landau - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (1):e13081.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 1, January 2022.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29. The Explanation Of Behaviour.C. Taylor - 1964 - Humanities Press.
  30.  19
    A spatial gradient in the strength of avoidance responses.R. Bugelski & N. E. Miller - 1938 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 23 (5):494.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  34
    Phenomenology, science, and geography: spatiality and the human sciences.John Pickles - 1985 - New York: Cambridge University Press.
    A work of outstanding originality and importance, which will become a cornerstone in the philosophy of geography, this book asks: What is human science? Is a truly human science of geography possible? What notions of spatiality adequately describe human spatial experience and behaviour? It sets out to answer these questions through a discussion of the nature of science in the human sciences, and, specifically, of the role of phenomenology in such inquiry. It criticises established understanding of phenomenology in these (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  32.  32
    Electronic behaviour settings for CSCW.Uta Pankoke-Babatz - 2000 - AI and Society 14 (1):3-30.
    Being present in the same room not only enables people to exchange non-verbal communication but also the physical properties of the room offer opportunities for action and thus contribute to the ongoing social process. This paper discusses concepts from social and behavioural sciences to better understand the role of physical environments and artefacts with respect to cooperation among a group of people. Barker's behaviour setting theory is studied and applied to electronic settings. Requirements for ‘electronic behaviour settings’ to enable situated (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  12
    Norms, Forms and Beds: Spatializing Sleep in Victorian Britain.Tom Crook - 2008 - Body and Society 14 (4):15-35.
    This article examines the spatialization of sleep in Victorian Britain across a range of institutions, including homes and dormitories. It situates the emergence of modern sleeping space at the intersection of two key narratives regarding the history of the body: Elias's `civilising process' and Foucault's account of the realization of a `disciplinary society'. Beginning in the early modern period, sleeping bodies were gradually accorded their own space set apart from others, and by the end of the 19th century the individual (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34. What’s So Spatial about Time Anyway?Sam Baron & Peter W. Evans - 2021 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 72 (1):159-183.
    Skow ([2007]), and much more recently Callender ([2017]), argue that time can be distinguished from space due to the special role it plays in our laws of nature: our laws determine the behaviour of physical systems across time, but not across space. In this work we assess the claim that the laws of nature might provide the basis for distinguishing time from space. We find that there is an obvious reason to be sceptical of the argument Skow submits for distinguishing (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  35.  9
    Human behavioural genetics of cognitive abilities and disabilities.Robert Plomin & Ian Craig - 1997 - Bioessays 19 (12):1117-1124.
    Although neither the genome nor the environment can be manipulated in research on human behaviour, some of the new tools of molecular genetics can be brought to bear on human behavioural disorders (e.g. cognitive disabilities) and quantitative traits (e.g. cognitive abilities). The inability to manipulate the human genome experimentally has had the positive effect of focusing attention on naturally occuring genetic variation responsible for behavioural differences among individuals in all their complex multifactorial splendour. Genes in such complex multiple‐gene systems are (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  6
    Response to spatial and nonspatial change in wild (WWCPS) and Wistar rats.Wojciech Pisula, Klaudia Modlińska & Rafał Stryjek - 2012 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 43 (2):124-131.
    Response to spatial and nonspatial change in wild and Wistar rats The purpose of the experiment was to investigate the effects of domestication on exploration in rats. The comparison was made between wild Warsaw-Wild-Captive-Pisula-Stryjek rats and Wistar laboratory rats. The study used a purpose-built maze divided into zones connected with a corridor. Objects were placed in two out of four zones. Their location and shape were subject to experimental manipulation. Transporter used to move rats to the maze provided the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  7
    The Hippocampal and Parietal Foundation of Spatial Cognition.N. Burgess (ed.) - 1998 - Oxford University Press UK.
    As we move around in our environment, and interact with it, many of the most important problems we face involve the processing of spatial information. We have to be able to navigate by perceiving and remembering the locations and orientations of the objects around us relative to ourself; we have to sense and act upon these objects; and we need to move through space to position ourselves in favourable locations or to avoid dangerous ones. While this appears so simple (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  38.  97
    Classical Behavior of the Dirac Bispinor.Sarah B. M. Bell, John P. Cullerne & Bernard M. Diaz - 2000 - Foundations of Physics 30 (1):35-57.
    It is usually supposed that the Dirac and radiation equations predict that the phase of a fermion will rotate through half the angle through which the fermion is rotated, which means, via the measured dynamical and geometrical phase factors, that the fermion must have a half-integral spin. We demonstrate that this is not the case and that the identical relativistic quantum mechanics can also be derived with the phase of the fermion rotating through the same angle as does the fermion (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  39.  10
    Childhood Experience Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Abilities: A Cross‐Cultural Study.Mariah G. Schug, Erica Barhorst-Cates, Jeanine Stefanucci, Sarah Creem-Regehr, Anna P. L. Olsen & Elizabeth Cashdan - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13096.
    Spatial experience in childhood is a factor in the development of spatial abilities. In this study, we assessed whether American and Faroese participants’ (N = 246, Mage = 19.31 years, 151 females) early spatial experience and adult spatial outcomes differed by gender and culture, and if early experience was related to adult performance and behavior. Participants completed retrospective reports on their childhood spatial experience, both large-scale (permitted childhood range size) and small-scale (Lego play). They (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  16
    A Non-Spatial Reality.Massimiliano Sassoli de Bianchi - 2020 - Foundations of Science 26 (1):143-170.
    It is generally assumed, and usually taken for granted, that reality is fully contained in space. However, when taking a closer look at the strange behavior of the entities of the micro-world, we are forced to abandon such a prejudice and recognize that space is just a temporary crystallization of a small theatre for reality, where the material entities can take a place and meet with each other. More precisely, phenomena like quantum entanglement, quantum interference effects and quantum indistinguishability, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  91
    A Perspectival Version of the Modal Interpretation of Quantum Mechanics and the Origin of Macroscopic Behavior.Gyula Bene & Dennis Dieks - 2001 - Foundations of Physics 32 (5):645-671.
    We study the process of observation (measurement), within the framework of a “perspectival” (“relational,” “relative state”) version of the modal interpretation of quantum mechanics. We show that if we assume certain features of discreteness and determinism in the operation of the measuring device (which could be a part of the observer's nerve system), this gives rise to classical characteristics of the observed properties, in the first place to spatial localization. We investigate to what extent semi-classical behavior of the (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  42.  29
    Conducting Interaction: Patterns of Behavior in Focused Encounters.Adam Kendon - 1990 - Cambridge University Press.
    This book makes available five classic studies of the organisation of behaviour in face-to-face interaction. It includes Adam Kendon's well-known study of gaze-direction in interaction, his study of greetings, of the interactional functions of facial expression and of the spatial organisation of naturally occurring interaction, as recorded by means of film or videotape. They represent some of the best work undertaken within the 'natural history' tradition of interaction studies, as originally formulated in the work of Bateson, Birdwhistell and Goffman. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  43.  27
    Reproductive Numbers for Nonautonomous Spatially Distributed Periodic SIS Models Acting on Two Time Scales.M. Marvá, R. Bravo de la Parra & P. Auger - 2011 - Acta Biotheoretica 60 (1):139-154.
    In this work we deal with a general class of spatially distributed periodic SIS epidemic models with two time scales. We let susceptible and infected individuals migrate between patches with periodic time dependent migration rates. The existence of two time scales in the system allows to describe certain features of the asymptotic behavior of its solutions with the help of a less dimensional, aggregated, system. We derive global reproduction numbers governing the general spatially distributed nonautonomous system through the aggregated (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  5
    Farmers’ behavioural determinants of on-farm biodiversity management in Europe: a systematic review.Fabian Klebl, Peter H. Feindt & Annette Piorr - forthcoming - Agriculture and Human Values:1-31.
    Agricultural intensification and landscape homogenisation are major drivers of biodiversity loss in European agricultural landscapes. Improvements require changes in farming practices, but empirical evidence on farmers’ motivations underlying their on-farm biodiversity management remains fragmented. To date, there is no aggregated overview of behavioural determinants that influence European farmers’ decisions to implement biodiversity-friendly farming practices. This study aims to fill this knowledge gap by conducting a systematic literature review of 150 empirical studies published between 2000 and 2022. We identified 108 potential (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  15
    Sex Differences in Exploration Behavior and the Relationship to Harm Avoidance.Kyle T. Gagnon, Elizabeth A. Cashdan, Jeanine K. Stefanucci & Sarah H. Creem-Regehr - 2016 - Human Nature 27 (1):82-97.
    Venturing into novel terrain poses physical risks to a female and her offspring. Females have a greater tendency to avoid physical harm, while males tend to have larger range sizes and often outperform females in navigation-related tasks. Given this backdrop, we expected that females would explore a novel environment with more caution than males, and that more-cautious exploration would negatively affect navigation performance. Participants explored a novel, large-scale, virtual environment in search of five objects, pointed in the direction of each (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  46.  8
    Dominance hierarchy and spatial distribution in captive red-capped mangabeys : Testing Hemelrijk’s agent-based model.Ruth Dolado & Francesc S. Beltran - 2011 - Interaction Studiesinteraction Studies Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 12 (3):461-473.
    We empirically tested Hemelrijk’s agent-based model, in which dyadic agonistic interaction between primate-group subjects determines their spatial distribution and whether or not the dominant subject has a central position with respect to the other subjects. We studied a group of captive red-capped mangabeys that met the optimal conditions for testing this model. We analyzed the spatial distribution of the subjects in relation to their rank in the dominance hierarchy and the results confirmed the validity of this model. In (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47. Learning to Communicate: The Emergence of Signaling in Spatialized Arrays of Neural Nets.Patrick Grim, Trina Kokalis & Paul St Denis - 2003 - Adaptive Behavior 10:45-70.
    We work with a large spatialized array of individuals in an environment of drifting food sources and predators. The behavior of each individual is generated by its simple neural net; individuals are capable of making one of two sounds and are capable of responding to sounds from their immediate neighbors by opening their mouths or hiding. An individual whose mouth is open in the presence of food is “fed” and gains points; an individual who fails to hide when a (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  48.  6
    Dominance hierarchy and spatial distribution in captive red-capped mangabeys.Ruth Dolado & Francesc S. Beltran - 2011 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 12 (3):461-473.
    We empirically tested Hemelrijk’s agent-based model, in which dyadic agonistic interaction between primate-group subjects determines their spatial distribution and whether or not the dominant subject has a central position with respect to the other subjects. We studied a group of captive red-capped mangabeys that met the optimal conditions for testing this model. We analyzed the spatial distribution of the subjects in relation to their rank in the dominance hierarchy and the results confirmed the validity of this model. In (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  35
    Mobile devices, designing affective spatialities.Luisa Paraguai - 2010 - Technoetic Arts 8 (2):221-228.
    This article concerns mobile technologies and the possibilities of engendering mediated presences, perceived as usual actions. Those devices have been embedded into the individual everyday practices, occupying personal spaces and making us share emotional and affective moments giving continuity to our anxiety and comprehension of the world. The theoretical approaches bring the understanding of playing and experiencing sensory states as enactive knowledge and Goffman's thoughts about co-temporality and users behaviours as social rituals. The bodyspace relation and the technological artefacts have (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  37
    The representation selection problem: Why we should favor the geometric-module framework of spatial reorientation over the view-matching framework.Alexandre Duval - 2019 - Cognition 192 (C):103985.
    Many species rely on the three-dimensional surface layout of an environment to find a desired goal following disorientation. They generally do so to the exclusion of other important spatial cues. Two influential frameworks for explaining that phenomenon are provided by geometric-module theories and view-matching theories of reorientation respectively. The former posit a module that operates only on representations of the global geo- metry of three-dimensional surfaces to guide behavior. The latter place snapshots, stored representations of the subject’s two-dimensional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 999