Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Putting anxiety in its place?Daniel P. Kimble - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):489-489.
  • Delay reduction: A field guide for optimal foragers?Peter R. Killeen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):341-342.
  • Hippocampus and memory for time.Raymond P. Kesner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):485-486.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Al Capone, discrete morphs, and complex dynamic systems.Douglas T. Kenrick & Stephanie Brown - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):560-561.
    We consider four mechanisms by which apparent discontinuities in the distribution of antisociality could arise: (1) executive genes or hormonal systems, (2) multiplicative interactions of predisposing factors, (3) environmental tracking into a limited number of social roles, and (4) cross-generational gene—environment interactions. A more explicit consideration of complex self-organizing dynamic systems may help us understand the maintenance of antisocial subpopulations.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Latent Profile Analysis of Affective Triggers for Risky and Impulsive Behavior.Emily Kemp, Naomi Sadeh & Arielle Baskin-Sommers - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dopamine and mental illness: Phenomenological and anatomical considerations.Ann E. Kelley - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):219-220.
  • Sensation seeking: A clarification, a caveat, and a conjecture.Richard J. Katz - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):443-443.
  • Parting's sweet sorrow: A pain pathway for the social sentiments?Leonard D. Katz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):435-436.
  • How long do relational representations in the hippocampus last during classical eyelid conditioning?Donald B. Katz & Joseph E. Steinmetz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):484-485.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Serotonin and aggression: What kind of relationship?Pierre Karli - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):340-340.
  • Where have all the peptides gone?Peter W. Kalivas - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):218-219.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rate of reinforcement matters in optimal foraging theory.Alejandro Kacelnik & John R. Krebs - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):340-341.
  • The neuropsychology of schizophrenia: Beyond the dopamine hypothesis to behavioural function.Michael H. Joseph - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):203-205.
  • Golly g: Interpreting Spearman's general factor.Lyle V. Jones - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):233-233.
  • Do we know enough about g to be able to speak of black–white differences?Ronald C. Johnson & Craig T. Nagoshi - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):232-233.
  • The nature of the black–white difference on various psychometric tests: Spearman's hypothesis.Arthur R. Jensen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):193-219.
  • The black–white difference in g: A phenomenon in search of a theory.Arthur R. Jensen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):246-263.
  • Representations as metaphiers.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):379-380.
  • A two-tiered theory of emotions: Affect and feeling.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):434-435.
  • The prefrontal cortex — accumbens circuit: Who's in charge?George E. Jaskiw & Daniel R. Weinberger - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):217-218.
  • On the hippocampus, time, and interference.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):503-504.
  • A call for greater concern regarding the underlying anatomy.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):483-484.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Excitatory amino acids, NMDA and sigma receptors: A role in schizophrenia?Karl L. R. Jansen & Richard L. M. Faull - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):34-35.
  • The evolution of hesitation, doubt, and map-making.D. T. D. James - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):488-489.
  • Science and subjective feelings.Dale Jamieson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):25-26.
  • Does our behavioral methodology conceal the deficit caused by hippocampal damage?David T. D. James - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):502-503.
  • Singer's intermediate conclusion.Frank Jackson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):24-25.
  • Emotion variables as personality traits.Carroll E. Izard - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):442-443.
  • From stimulus-bound emotive command systems to drive-free emotions.C. E. Izard - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):433-434.
  • Integrating the literature on anxiety, memory, and the hippocampus.Susan D. Iversen - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):487-488.
  • Information synthesis in cortical areas as an important link in brain mechanisms of mind.Alexei M. Ivanitsky - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):686-687.
    To explore the mechanism of sensation correlations between EP component amplitude and signal detection indices were studied. The time of sensation coincided with the peak latency of those EP components that showed a correlation with both indices. The components presumably reflected information synthesis in projection cortical neurons. A mechanism providing the synthesis process is proposed.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mind – your head!R. P. Ingvaldsen & H. T. A. Whiting - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):685-686.
    Gray takes an information-processing paradigm as his departure point, invoking a comparator as part of the system. He concludes that consciousness is to be found “in” the comparator but is unable to point to how the comparison takes place. Thus, the comparator turns out not to be an entity arising out of brain research per se, but out of the logic of the paradigm. In this way, Gray both reinvents dualism and remains trapped in the language game of his own (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A neuropsychology of psychosis.Loring J. Ingraham - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):34-34.
  • Perspective, reflection, transparent explanation, and other minds.S. L. Hurley - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):684-685.
    Perspective and reflection have each been considered in some way basic to phenomenal consciousness. Each has possible evolutionary value, though neither seems sufficient for consciousness. Consider an account of consciousness in terms of the combination of perspective and reflection, its relationship to the problem of other minds, and its capacity to inherit evolutionary explanation from its components.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Going from task descriptions to memory structures.Michael S. Humphreys & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):483-483.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Comparative cognition revisited.Stewart H. Hulse - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):379-379.
  • Temporal discontiguity: Alternative to, or component of, existing theories of hippocampal function?Donna J. Hughey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):501-502.
  • Experimental investigation of animal suffering.B. O. Hughes & J. C. Petherick - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):23-24.
  • Genes, hormones, and gender in sociopathy.Katharine Hoyenga - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):560-560.
    Although serotonin, testosterone, and genes contribute to sociopathy, the relationships are probably indirect and subject to modifiers (e.g., present only under certain conditions of rearing and temperament). Age at menarche may be a marker variable as well as a causal factor. Since the genders differ in all four areas, sex differences in sociopathy represent a very complex interaction of these factors.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Choice and preference-you can't always want what you get.Alasdair I. Houston - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):339-340.
  • The localization of general memory functions.James A. Horel - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):482-482.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Implications of an evolutionary biopsychosocial model.Harmon R. Holcomb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):559-560.
    Mealey's work has several interesting implications: It refutes the charge that sociobiology paints a cynical portrait of human nature and adopts a one-sided reductionism; it exemplifies a general theoretical scheme for constructing evolutionary biopsychosocial models of human behavior; and it has the practical effect of promoting and informing early intervention in children at risk for psychopathic disorder.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The mechanism of positive symptoms in schizophrenia.Ralph E. Hoffman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):33-34.
  • Fears, phobias and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.Arne Öhman & Susan Mineka - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (3):483-522.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   239 citations  
  • Differential Effects of Infant Vocalizations on Approach-Avoidance Postural Movements in Mothers.Daiki Hiraoka, Yuuki Ooishi, Ryoko Mugitani & Michio Nomura - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology.John R. Hibbing, Kevin B. Smith & John R. Alford - 2014 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37 (3):297-307.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • A cardinal principle for neuropsychology, with implications for schizophrenia and mania.David Hestenes - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):31-32.
  • The neuropsychology of schizophrenia: Act 3.D. R. Hemsley, J. N. P. Rawlins, J. Feldon, S. H. Jones & J. A. Gray - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):209-215.
  • Psychopathology and the discontinuity of conscious experience.David R. Hemsley - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):683-684.
    It is accepted that “primary awareness” may emerge from the integration of two classes of information. It is unclear, however, why this cannot take place within the comparator rather than in conjunction with feedback to the perceptual systems. The model has plausibility in relation to the continuity of conscious experience in the normal waking state and may be extended to encompass certain aspects of the “sense of self” which are frequently disrupted in psychotic patients.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Panksepp's psychobiological theory of emotions: Some substantiation.Robert G. Heath - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):432-433.