Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Animals, science, and morality.R. G. Frey - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):22-22.
  • Concepts of suffering in veterinary science.Andrew F. Fraser - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):21-22.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Taking the animal's viewpoint seriously.Michael Allen Fox - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):20-21.
  • Real-Time Elicitation of Moral Emotions Using a Prejudice Paradigm.Melike M. Fourie, Nadine Kilchenmann, Susan Malcolm-Smith & Kevin G. F. Thomas - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On seeking the mythical fountain of consciousness.Jeffrey Foss - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):682-682.
    Because consciousness has an organizational, or functional, center, Gray supposes that there must be a corresponding physical center in the brain. He proposes further that since this center generates consciousness, ablating it would eliminate consciousness, while leaving behavior intact. But the center of consciousness is simply the product of the functional linkages among sensory input, memory, inner speech, and so on, and behavior.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Emotions are objective events.Elzbieta Fonberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):429-430.
  • The epigenesis of sociopathy.Aurelio José Figueredo - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):556-557.
    Mealey distinguishes two types of sociopathy: (1) or obligate, and (2) or facultative. Either sociopathy evolved twice, or one form is derived from the other, e.g., through: (1) genetic assimilation generating polymorphism in the relative strength of biases favoring the development of otherwise facultative strategies, or (2) independently heritable but strategically relevant characteristics biasing the optimal selection of facultative strategies.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural circuit models of psychopathology: Dancing on the precipice of neuromythology?H. C. Fibiger - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):212-213.
  • Transplantation, plasticity, and the aging host.David L. Felten - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):58-58.
    Neural transplantation as a recovery strategy for neuro-degenerative diseases in humans has used mainly grafting following acute denervation strategies in young adult hosts. Our work in aged mice and rats demonstrates an age-related increase in susceptibility to oxidative damage from neurotoxins, a remarkably poor recovery of C57BL/6 mice from MPTP insult with transplantation and growth factors, even at 12 months of age, and diminished plasticity of host neurons. We believe that extrapolation of data from young adult animal models to aged (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The delay-reduction hypothesis: A choice solution.Edmund Fantino & Nureya Abarca - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):350-362.
  • Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis.Edmund Fantino & Nureya Abarca - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):315-330.
  • The ghost in the machine: What if the midbrain output is excitatory?James H. Fallon - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):210-212.
  • The comparative approach in personality study.H. J. Eysenck - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):440-441.
  • The nature of cognitive differences between blacks and whites.H. J. Eysenck - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):229-229.
  • Psychopathology: Type or trait?H. J. Eysenck - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):555-556.
    Mealey proposes two categorical classes of sociopath, primary and secondary. I criticize this distinction on the basis that constructs of this kind have proved unrealistic in personality taxonomy and that dimensional systems capture reality much more successfully. I suggest how such a system could work in this particular context.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are sensation-seeking behavior, sleep patterns, and brain plasticity related?Vesna A. Eterović & P. A. Ferchmin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):439-440.
  • Bridging Classical and Revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory Research: A Longitudinal Analysis of a Large Population Study.Daniela A. Espinoza Oyarce, Richard Burns, Peter Butterworth & Nicolas Cherbuin - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The reinforcement sensitivity theory proposes that neurobiological systems mediate protective and appetitive behaviours and the functioning of these systems is associated to personality traits. In this manner, the RST is a link between neuroscience, behaviour, and personality. The theory evolved to the present revised version describing three systems: fight-flight-freezing, behavioural approach/activation, and behavioural inhibition. However, the most widely available measure of the theory, the BIS/BAS scales, only investigates two systems. Using a large longitudinal community survey, we found that the BIS/BAS (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Sociopathy and sociobiology: Biological units and behavioral units.Carl J. Erickson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):555-555.
    Behavioral biologists have long sought to link behavioral units (e.g., aggression, depression, sociopathy) with biological units (e.g., genes, neurotransmitters, hormones, neuroanatomical loci). These units, originally contrived for descriptive purposes, often lead to misunderstandings when they are reified for purposes of causal analysis. This genetic and biochemical explanation for sociopathy reflects such problems.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representation: A concept that fills no gaps.Robert Epstein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):377-378.
  • Extending arousal theory and reflecting on biosocial approaches to social science.Lee Ellis - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):554-554.
    This commentary extends arousal theory to suggest an explanation for the well-established inverse correlation between church attendance and involvement in crime. In addition, the results of two surveys of social scientists are reviewed to reveal just how little impact the biosocial/sociobiological perspective has had thus far on social science.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Context and consciousness.Colin G. Ellard - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):681-682.
    The commentary argues that we cannot be sure that human consciousness has survival value and that in order to understand the origins and, perhaps, the function of consciousness, we should examine the behavioural and neural precursors to consciousness in nonhumans. An example is given of research on the role of context in decisions regarding fleeing from probable predators in the Mongolian gerbil.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • A focalized deficit within an elegant system.Irene J. Elkins & Rue L. Cromwell - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):27-28.
  • The role of emotion in sociopathy: Contradictions and unanswered questions.Nancy Eisenberg - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):553-554.
    Emotion is critical in Mealey's conceptual arguments. However, several of her assertions about the role of emotion in sociopathy are problematic. Questions are raised regarding the link between lack of anxiety and low levels of secondary emotions such as love and sympathy, the argument that sociopaths are low in anxiety but high in neuroticism, and the designation of anxiety as a secondary emotion.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampal memory system and its functional comments: Further explication and clarification.Howard Eichenbaum, Tim Otto & Neal J. Cohen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):500-517.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system.Howard Eichenbaum, Tim Otto & Neal J. Cohen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):449-472.
    There is considerable evidence that the hippocampal system contributes both to the temporary maintenance of memories and to the processing of a particular type of memory representation. The findings on amnesia suggest that these two distinguishing features of hippocampal memory processing are orthogonal. Together with anatomical and physiological data, the neuropsychological findings support a model of cortico-hippocampal interactions in which the temporal and representational properties of hippocampal memory processing are mediated separately. We propose that neocortical association areas maintain shortterm memories (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   143 citations  
  • The hippocampal system: Dissociating its functional components and recombining them in the service of declarative memory.Howard Eichenbaum, Tim Otto & Neal J. Cohen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):772-776.
    Continuing commentary raised several issues concerning our proposal that the hippocampus, parahippocampal region, and cortical association areas mediate different aspects of memory function. Recent relevant findings strengthen our argument that neocortical areas and the parahippocampal region maintain persistent encodings of specific single items and that the hippocampus mediates representations of the relations among these items. The reciprocally and closely interconnected structures that compose the hippocampal memory system work interactively to support flexible memory expression that is relevant to the natural behavior (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness, memory, and the hippocampal system: What kind of connections can we make?Howard Eichenbaum & Neal J. Cohen - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):680-681.
    Gray's account is remarkable in its depth and scope but too little attention is paid to poor correspondences with the literature on hippocampal/subicular damage, the theta rhythm, and novelty detection. An alternative account, focusing on hippocampal involvement in organizing memories in a way that makes them accessible to conscious recollection but not in access to consciousness per se, avoids each of these limitations.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dopaminergic excess or dysregulation?Terrence S. Early, John Wayne Haller & Michael Posner - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):26-26.
  • The philosophical foundations of animal welfare.John Dupré - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):19-20.
  • Multiple potential mechanisms of graft action is not a new idea.Stephen B. Dunnett - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):56-57.
    It is well established that neural grafts can exert functional effects on the host animal by a multiplicity of different mechanisms – by diffuse release of trophic molecules, neurohormones, and deficient neurotransmitters, as well as by growth and reformation of neural circuits. Our challenge is to understand how these different mechanisms complement each other.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Elegant studies of transplant-derived repair of cognitive performance.Stephen B. Dunnett & Eduardo M. Torres - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):57-57.
    Cholinergic-rich grafts have been shown to be effective in restoring maze-learning deficits in rats with lesions of the forebrain cholinergic projection system. However, the relevance of those studies to developing novel therapies for Alzheimer's disease is questioned.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The informational character of representations.Fred Dretske - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):376-377.
  • A neuropsychology of deception and self-deception.Roger A. Drake - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):552-553.
    As more criminals are imprisoned, other individuals change their behavior to replace them, as predicted by the theory of strategic behavior. The physiological correlates of sociopathy suggest that research in cognitive neuroscience can lead toward a solution. Promising pathways include building upon current knowledge of self-deceit, the independence of positive and negative emotions, the lateralization of risk and caution, and the conditions promoting prosocial behavior.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How does the physiology change with symptom exacerbation and remission in schizophrenia?George G. Dougherty, Stuart R. Steinhauer, Joseph Zubin & Daniel P. van Kammen - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):25-26.
  • Epistemology, ethics, and evolution.Strachan Donnelley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):18-19.
  • The integrative power of the CS-US interval in other contexts.James A. Dinsmoor - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):336-337.
  • Commentary: Primary Emotional Systems and Personality: An Evolutionary Perspective.Stefano I. Di Domenico & Richard M. Ryan - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Introspection as the Rosetta stone: Millstone or fifth wheel?Ronald de Sousa - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):428-429.
  • Ingestion and emotional health.Nancy K. Dess - 1991 - Human Nature 2 (3):235-269.
    Evidence abounds of a close relation between ingestive and affective processes in rats and in humans. Emotional distress alters food intake and body weight; conversely, alterations in eating and weight influence emotional health. Thorough experimental analysis of the ingestion-affect relation may clarify the mechanisms of anxiety and depression. A strategy is proposed for examination of environmental and dispositional determinants of ingestive processes, emotionality, and responses to stress.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Relating experience to the brain.Joseph de Rivera - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):427-428.
  • Possible roles for a predictor plus comparator mechanism in human episodic recognition memory and imitative learning.Simon Dennis & Michael Humphreys - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):678-679.
    This commentary is divided into two parts. The first considers a possible role for Gray's predictor plus comparator mechanism in human episodic recognition memory. It draws on the computational specifications of recognition outlined in Humphreys et al. to demonstrate how the logically necessary components of recognition tasks might be mapped onto the mechanism. The second part demonstrates how the mechanism outlined by Gray might be implicated in a form of imitative learning suitable for the acquisition of complex tasks.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Overworking the hippocampus.Daniel C. Dennett - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):677-678.
    Gray mistakenly thinks I have rejected the sort of theoretical enterprise he is undertaking, because, according to him, I think that "more data" is all that is needed to resolve all the issues. Not at all. My stalking horse was the bizarre (often pathetic) claim that no amount of empirical, "third-person point-of-view" science (data plus theory) could ever reduce the residue of mystery about consciousness to zero. This "New Mysterianism" (Flanagan, 1991) is one that he should want to combat as (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Memory: A matter of fitness.Juan D. Delius - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):375-376.
  • Animal and human emotionality.José M. R. Delgado - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):425-427.
  • On Singer: More argument, less prescriptivism.David DeGrazia - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):18-18.
  • Multiple 5-HT systems and multiple punishment processes.J. F. W. Deakin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):337-338.
  • A physiological basis for hippocampal involvement in coding temporally discontiguous events.Sam A. Deadwyler - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):500-501.
  • Hunting for consciousness in the brain: What is (the name of) the game?José-Luis Díaz - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):679-680.
    Robust theories concerning the connection between consciousness and brain function should derive not only from empirical evidence but also from a well grounded inind-body ontology. In the case of the comparator hypothesis, Gray develops his ideas relying extensively on empirical evidence, but he bounces irresolutely among logically incompatible metaphysical theses which, in turn, leads him to excessively skeptical conclusions concerning the naturalization of consciousness.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Other minds and other species.Marian Stamp Dawkins - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):49-61.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Heterogeneity, orienting and habituation in schizophrenia.Michael E. Dawson & Erin A. Hazlett - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):24-25.