Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Talking to yourself about what is where: What is the vocabulary of preattentive vision?Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):254-255.
  • “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.
    Fundamental to spatial knowledge in all species are the representations underlying object recognition, object search, and navigation through space. But what sets humans apart from other species is our ability to express spatial experience through language. This target article explores the language ofobjectsandplaces, asking what geometric properties are preserved in the representations underlying object nouns and spatial prepositions in English. Evidence from these two aspects of language suggests there are significant differences in the geometric richness with which objects and places (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   100 citations  
  • Cognition and Content.João Branquinho - 2005 - Lisboa, Portugal: Centro de Filosofia da Universidade de Lisboa.
    Os tópicos e problemas filosóficos discutidos no volume são de natureza bastante variada: a natureza da complexidade computacional no processamento de uma língua natural; a relação entre o significado linguístico e o sentido Fregeano; as conexões entre a a agência e o poder; o conteúdo semântico da ficção; a explicação dos impasses éticos; a natureza dos argumentos cépticos; as conexões entre as dissociações cognitivas e o carácter modular da mente; a relação entre a referência e o significado. Estes tópicos deixam-se (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.
  • What counts as local?Andrew W. Young - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):88-89.
  • Visual working memory capacity for objects from different categories: A face-specific maintenance effect.Jason H. Wong, Matthew S. Peterson & James C. Thompson - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):719-731.
  • Models of atypical development must also be models of normal development.Gert Westermann & Denis Mareschal - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):771-772.
    Connectionist models aiming to reveal the mechanisms of atypical development must in their undamaged form constitute plausible models of normal development and follow a developmental trajectory that matches empirical data. Constructivist models that adapt their structure to the learning task satisfy this demand. They are therefore more informative in the study of atypical development than the static models employed by Thomas & Karmiloff-Smith (T&K-S).
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.
  • From observations on language to theories of visual perception.Johan Wagemans - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):253-254.
  • What do double dissociations prove?G. Van Orden - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (1):111-172.
    Brain damage may doubly dissociate cognitive modules, but the practice of revealing dissociations is predicated on modularity being true (T. Shallice, 1988). This article questions the utility of assuming modularity, as it examines a paradigmatic double dissociation of reading modules. Reading modules illustrate two general problems. First, modularity fails to converge on a fixed set of exclusionary criteria that define pure cases. As a consequence, competing modular theories force perennial quests for purer cases, which simply perpetuates growth in the list (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • The symbolic brain or the invisible hand?René van Hezewijk & Edward H. F. de Haan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):85-86.
  • Playing Flourens to Fodor's Gall.Tim van Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-84.
  • Prosopagnosia, conscious awareness and the interactive brain.Robert Van Gulick - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-85.
  • Naming and Knowing Revisited: Eyetracking Correlates of Anomia in Progressive Aphasia.Molly B. Ungrady, Maurice Flurie, Bonnie M. Zuckerman, Daniel Mirman & Jamie Reilly - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  • The functional architecture of visual attention may still be modular.Carlo Umiltà - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):82-83.
  • Prepositions aren't places.Barbara Tversky & Herbert H. Clark - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):252-253.
  • Defining a Conceptual Topography of Word Concreteness: Clustering Properties of Emotion, Sensation, and Magnitude among 750 English Words.Joshua Troche, Sebastian J. Crutch & Jamie Reilly - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • What Makes Us Conscious?Anthony P. Atkinson & Michael S. C. Thomas - 1999 - Journal of Intelligent Systems 9 (5-6):307-354.
  • Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling.Michael Thomas & Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):727-750.
    It is often assumed that similar domain-specific behavioural impairments found in cases of adult brain damage and developmental disorders correspond to similar underlying causes, and can serve as convergent evidence for the modular structure of the normal adult cognitive system. We argue that this correspondence is contingent on an unsupported assumption that atypical development can produce selective deficits while the rest of the system develops normally (Residual Normality), and that this assumption tends to bias data collection in the field. Based (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • From perception to cognition.Michael J. Tarr - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):251-252.
  • Recognition and categorization of biologically significant objects by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the domain of food.Elizabeth Spelke - 2001 - Cognition 82 (2):127-155.
  • Alternative strategies of categorization.Edward E. Smith, Andrea L. Patalano & John Jonides - 1998 - Cognition 65 (2-3):167-196.
  • The real functional architecture is gray, wet and slippery.Steven L. Small - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):81-82.
  • Is spatial language a special case?Dan I. Slobin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):249-251.
  • Semantic Boost on Episodic Associations: An Empirically‐Based Computational Model.Yaron Silberman, Shlomo Bentin & Risto Miikkulainen - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (4):645-671.
    Words become associated following repeated co-occurrence episodes. This process might be further determined by the semantic characteristics of the words. The present study focused on how semantic and episodic factors interact in incidental formation of word associations. First, we found that human participants associate semantically related words more easily than unrelated words; this advantage increased linearly with repeated co-occurrence. Second, we developed a computational model, SEMANT, suggesting a possible mechanism for this semantic-episodic interaction. In SEMANT, episodic associations are implemented through (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Throwing out the neuropsychological data with the locality bathwater?Philip Servos & Elizabeth M. Olds - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-81.
  • Locus-pocus.Carlo Semenza - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-80.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perception and its interactive substrate: Psychophysical linking hypotheses and psychophysical methods.Robert Sekuler - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):79-79.
  • Recognition and categorization of biologically significant objects by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the domain of food.Laurie R. Santos, Marc D. Hauser & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2001 - Cognition 82 (2):127-155.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Structure and Deterioration of Semantic Memory: A Neuropsychological and Computational Investigation.Timothy T. Rogers, Matthew A. Lambon Ralph, Peter Garrard, Sasha Bozeat, James L. McClelland, John R. Hodges & Karalyn Patterson - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (1):205-235.
  • Précis of semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach.Timothy T. Rogers & James L. McClelland - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):689-714.
    In this prcis we focus on phenomena central to the reaction against similarity-based theories that arose in the 1980s and that subsequently motivated the approach to semantic knowledge. Specifically, we consider (1) how concepts differentiate in early development, (2) why some groupings of items seem to form or coherent categories while others do not, (3) why different properties seem central or important to different concepts, (4) why children and adults sometimes attest to beliefs that seem to contradict their direct experience, (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Parallel Distributed Processing at 25: Further Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition.Timothy T. Rogers & James L. McClelland - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1024-1077.
    This paper introduces a special issue of Cognitive Science initiated on the 25th anniversary of the publication of Parallel Distributed Processing (PDP), a two-volume work that introduced the use of neural network models as vehicles for understanding cognition. The collection surveys the core commitments of the PDP framework, the key issues the framework has addressed, and the debates the framework has spawned, and presents viewpoints on the current status of these issues. The articles focus on both historical roots and contemporary (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • A simple model from a powerful framework that spans levels of analysis.Timothy T. Rogers & James L. McClelland - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):729-749.
    The commentaries reflect three core themes that pertain not just to our theory, but to the enterprise of connectionist modeling more generally. The first concerns the relationship between a cognitive theory and an implemented computer model. Specifically, how does one determine, when a model departs from the theory it exemplifies, whether the departure is a useful simplification or a critical flaw? We argue that the answer to this question depends partially upon the model's intended function, and we suggest that connectionist (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Local and distributed processes in attentional orienting.Michael I. Posner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):78-79.
  • Parallel distributed processing challenges the strong modularity hypothesis, not the locality assumption.David C. Plaut - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):77-78.
  • A Computational and Empirical Investigation of Graphemes in Reading.Conrad Perry, Johannes C. Ziegler & Marco Zorzi - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (5):800-828.
    It is often assumed that graphemes are a crucial level of orthographic representation above letters. Current connectionist models of reading, however, do not address how the mapping from letters to graphemes is learned. One major challenge for computational modeling is therefore developing a model that learns this mapping and can assign the graphemes to linguistically meaningful categories such as the onset, vowel, and coda of a syllable. Here, we present a model that learns to do this in English for strings (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “Shallow Draughts Intoxicate the Brain”: Lessons from Cognitive Science for Cognitive Neuropsychology.Karalyn Patterson & David C. Plaut - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):39-58.
    This article presents a sobering view of the discipline of cognitive neuropsychology as practiced over the last three or four decades. Our judgment is that, although the study of abnormal cognition resulting from brain injury or disease in previously normal adults has produced a catalogue of fascinating and highly selective deficits, it has yielded relatively little advance in understanding how the brain accomplishes its cognitive business. We question the wisdom of the following three “choices” in mainstream cognitive neuropsychology: (a) single‐case (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • What do double dissociations prove?Guy C. Orden, Bruce F. Pennington & Gregory O. Stone - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (1):111-172.
    Brain damage may doubly dissociate cognitive modules, but the practice of revealing dissociations is predicated on modularity being true (T. Shallice, 1988). This article questions the utility of assuming modularity, as it examines a paradigmatic double dissociation of reading modules. Reading modules illustrate two general problems. First, modularity fails to converge on a fixed set of exclusionary criteria that define pure cases. As a consequence, competing modular theories force perennial quests for purer cases, which simply perpetuates growth in the list (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Spatial development.David R. Olson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):249-249.
  • Computational levels again.Mike Oaksford - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):76-77.
  • Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects.Jong-Yoon Myung, Sheila E. Blumstein & Julie C. Sedivy - 2006 - Cognition 98 (3):223-243.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Distributed locality and large-scale neurocognitive networks.M. Marsel Mesulam - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):74-76.
  • On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning.Ken McRae, Virginia R. de Sa & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1997 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 126 (2):99-130.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  • Integrating conceptual knowledge within and across representational modalities.Chris McNorgan, Jackie Reid & Ken McRae - 2011 - Cognition 118 (2):211-233.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Neuropsychology: Going loco?Rosaleen A. McCarthy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):73-74.
  • Letting structure emerge: connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition.James L. McClelland, Matthew M. Botvinick, David C. Noelle, David C. Plaut, Timothy T. Rogers, Mark S. Seidenberg & Linda B. Smith - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8):348-356.
  • Names, concepts, features and the living/nonliving things dissociation.J. Frederico Marques - 2002 - Cognition 85 (3):251-275.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Distinguishing the linguistic from the sublinguistic and the objective from the configurational.Scott D. Mainwaring - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):248-249.
  • Whence and whither in spatial language and spatial cognition?Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):255-265.
  • Do neuropsychologists think in terms of interactive models?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):72-73.