Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The hippocampus: Relational processor or antiprocessor?Neil McNaughton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):487-488.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The hippocampus, synaptic enhancement, and intermediate-term memory.B. L. McNaughton & C. A. Barnes - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):507-508.
  • Optimal foraging for operant conditioners.James N. McNair - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):343-344.
  • Is the hippocampus a store, intermediate or otherwise?Neil McNaughton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):508-509.
  • Gray's Neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of septohippocampal theories.Neil McNaughton - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):492-493.
  • Suffering by analogy.David McFarland - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):27-27.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Diathesis stress model or “Just So” story?Richard M. McFall, James T. Townsend & Richard J. Viken - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):565-566.
    Mealey's sociopathy model is an exemplar of popular diathesis-stress models. Although such models, when presented in descriptive language, offer the illusion of integrative explanation, their actual scientific value is very limited because they fail to make specific, quantitative, falsifiable predictions. Conceptual and quantitative weaknesses of such diathesis-stress models are discussed and the requirements for useful models are outlined.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What exactly do amnesics fail to store normally?Andrew R. Mayes - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):486-487.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genetic issues in “the sociobiology of sociopathy”.Stephen C. Maxson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):565-565.
    A consideration of the genetics of sociopathy suggests the following. The author's Evolutionary Stable Strategy (ESS) types 2 to 4 are more likely than types 1 and 5 in crimes of violence, and there may not be an ESS for crimes of property or for sociopathy. Correlations between sociopathy and crimes of property are also more likely due to environmental than to genetic variants, and correlations between sociopathy and crimes of property are due more to environmental than genetic variants.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The noradrenergic locus coeruleus–the center of attention?Stephen T. Mason - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):445-445.
  • Noradrenaline: Attention or anxiety?Stephen T. Mason - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):491-492.
  • Memory processing by the brain: Subregionalization, species-dependency, and network character.Hans J. Markowitsch - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):506-507.
  • A la représentation du temps perdu.John C. Marshall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):382-383.
  • Approximations to a neuropsychological model of schizophrenia.Theo C. Manschreck & Brendan A. Maher - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):36-37.
  • Three-store theories of memory.William S. Maki - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):505-506.
  • Obtaining and applying objective criteria in animal welfare.Anne E. Magurran - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):26-27.
  • Hidden adaptationism.David Magnus & Peter Thiel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):26-26.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sociobiology, sociopathy, and social policy.Richard Machalek - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):564-564.
    Evolutionary analysis suggests that policies based on deterrence may cope effectively with primary sociopathy if the threat of punishment fits the crime in the cost/benefit calculus of the sociopath, not that of the public. On the other hand, policies designed to offset serious disadvantage in social competition may help inhibit the development of secondary sociopathy, rather than deter its expression.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Comparative studies of animal intelligence: Is Spearman's g really Hull's D?Euan M. Macphail - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):234-235.
  • Some questions of strategy in neuropsychological research on anxiety.William Lyons - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):490-491.
  • Concerning the alleged four basic emotions.William Lyons - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):440-441.
  • Priming determinist beliefs diminishes implicit components of self-agency.Margaret T. Lynn, Paul S. Muhle-Karbe, Henk Aarts & Marcel Brass - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
  • Neural transplantation, cognitive aging and speech.Michael P. Lynch - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):62-63.
    Research on neural transplantation has great potential societal importance in part because of the expanding proportion of the population that is elderly. Transplantation studies can benefit from the guidance of research on cognitive aging, especially in connection with the assessment of behavioral outcomes. Speech for example, might be explored using avian models.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fatherless rearing leads to sociopathy.David T. Lykken - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):563-564.
    Endorsing Mealey's analysis, it is pointed out that increasing rates of crime and violence are due to increasing proportions of children being reared in circumstances radically different from the extendedfamily environment to which we are evolntionarily adapted, that is, they are reared without fathers.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Introspection and cultural knowledge systems.Catherine Lutz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):439-440.
  • On incest and mathematical modeling.C. J. Lumsden & E. O. Wilson - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):742.
  • Schizophrenia and attention: In and out of context.R. E. Lubow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):35-36.
  • Human consciousness: One of a kind.R. E. Lubow - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):689-689.
    To avoid teleological interpretations, it is important to make a distinction between functions and uses of consciousness, and to address questions concerning the consequences of consciousness. Assumptions about the phylogenetic distribution of consciousness are examined. It is concluded that there is some value in identifying consciousness an exclusively human attribute.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Feeling of Action Tendencies: On the Emotional Regulation of Goal-Directed Behavior.Robert Lowe & Tom Ziemke - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
  • Toward a theory of moods.Eric Lormand - 1985 - Philosophical Studies 47 (May):385-407.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Cognitive psychology's representation of behaviorism.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):381-382.
  • Correlating mind and body.T. J. Lioyd-Jones, N. Donnelly & B. Weekes - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):688-688.
    Gray's integration of the different levels of description and explanation in his theory is problematic: The introduction of consciousness into his theorising consists of the mind-brain identity assumption, which tells us nothing new. There need not be correlations between levels of description. Gray's account does not extend beyond “brute” correlation. Integration must be achieved in a principled, mutually constraining way.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intraretrosplenial grafts of cholinergic neurons and spatial memory function.Ying J. Li & Walter C. Low - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):61-62.
    The transplantation of cholinergic neurons into the hippocampal formation has been well characterized. We describe our studies on the effects of cholinergic transplants in the retrosplenial cortex. These transplants were capable of ameliorating spatial navigation deficits in rats with septohippocampal lesions. In addition, we provide evidence for the modulation of transplanted neurons by the host brain.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Volitional processes in relation to the SMA.Benjamin Libet - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):592-594.
  • Conditioned suppression and behavioural inhibition.Julian C. Leslie - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):489-490.
  • Toward a neurological psychiatry.Michel Le Moal - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):221-222.
  • Optimality: Sequences, variability, learning.S. E. G. Lea - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):343-343.
  • Psychobiology without psychosocial significance.Richard S. Lazarus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):438-439.
  • Memory representations in animals: Some metatheoretical issues.Roy Lachman & Janet L. Lachman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):380-381.
  • Alternative approaches to the psychology of foraging.John M. Kruse - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):342-343.
  • The cognitive regulation of emotions: The role of success versus failure experience and coping dispositions.Heinz Walter Krohne, Manuela Pieper, Nina Knoll & Nadine Breimer - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (2):217-243.
  • An evaluation of Mealey's hypotheses based on psychopathy checklist: Identified groups.David S. Kosson & Joseph P. Newman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):562-563.
    Although Mealey's account provides several interesting hypotheses, her integration across disparate samples renders the value of her explanation for psychopathy ambiguous. Recent evidence on Psychopathy Checklist-identified samples (Hare, 1991) suggests primary emotional and cognitive deficits inconsistent with her model. Whereas high-anxious psychopaths display interpersonal deficits consistent with Mealey's hypotheses, low-anxious psychopaths' deficits appear more sensitive to situational parameters than predicted.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The starting function of the SMA.H. H. Kornhuber & L. Deecke - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):591-592.
  • Searching for a technology of behavior.Bryan Kolb, W. J. Jacobs & Bruce Petrie - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):220-221.
  • The nature of psychometric g.Paul Kline - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):234-234.
  • Generality and specifics in psychobiological theory of emotions.Eric Klinger & Ernest D. Kemble - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):437-438.
  • Panic, separation anxiety, and endorphins.Donald F. Klein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):436-437.
  • Beyond the answer: Post-error processes.Gernot D. Kleiter & Kurt Schwarzenbacher - 1989 - Cognition 32 (3):255-277.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Septohippocampal comparator: Consciousness generator or attention feedback loop?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):687-688.
    As Gray insists, his comparator model proposes a brute correlation only – of consciousness with septohippocampal output. I suggest that the comparator straddles a feedback loop that boosts the activation ofnovelrepresentations, thus helping them feature in present or recollected experience. Such a role in organizing conscious contents would transcend correlation and help explain how consciousness emerges from brain function.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Sharpening the focus on functions of the hippocampus.Daniel P. Kimble - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):504-505.