Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Image or neural coding of inner speech and agency?Gail Zivin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):534-535.
  • Abstract codes are not just for chimpanzees.Thomas R. Zentall - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):157-158.
  • Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.
  • How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.
  • The pros and cons of having a word for it.S. F. Walker - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):156-157.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Younger apes and human children plan their moves in a maze task.Christoph J. Völter & Josep Call - 2014 - Cognition 130 (2):186-203.
  • The organization of expert systems, a tutorial.Mark Stefik, Jan Aikins, Robert Balzer, John Benoit, Lawrence Birnbaum, Frederick Hayes-Roth & Earl Sacerdoti - 1982 - Artificial Intelligence 18 (2):135-173.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.
  • Planning and meta-planning.Mark Stefik - 1981 - Artificial Intelligence 16 (2):141-169.
  • Hallucinations and contextually generated interpretations.Nicholas P. Spanos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):533-534.
  • A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
  • Everyday planning: An analysis of daily time management.Daniel J. Simons & Kathleen M. Galotti - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (1):61-64.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.
  • Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.
  • Hallucination, rationalization, and response set.Steven Schwartz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):532-533.
  • Everyday Activities.Holger Schultheis & Richard P. Cooper - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (2):214-222.
    Topics in Cognitive Science, Volume 14, Issue 2, Page 214-222, April 2022.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Beyond the Purely Cognitive: Belief Systems, Social Cognitions, and Metacognitions As Driving Forces in Intellectual Performance.Alan H. Schoenfeld - 1983 - Cognitive Science 7 (4):329-363.
    This study explores the way that belief systems, interactions with social or experimental environments, and skills at the “control” level in decision‐making shape people's behavior as they solve problems. It is argued that problem‐solvers' beliefs (not necessarily consciously held) about what is useful in mathematics may determine the set of “cognitive resources” at their disposal as they do mathematics. Such beliefs may, for example, render inaccessible to them large bodies of information that are stored in long‐term memory and that are (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.
  • Verbal hallucinations and information processing.Bjørn Rishovd Rund - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):531-532.
  • Does language training affect the code used by chimpanzees?: Some cautions and reservations.H. L. Roitblat - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):155-156.
  • The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.
  • Doubts about the importance of language training and the abstract code.William A. Roberts - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-155.
  • Program Structure and Design.Robert S. Rist - 1995 - Cognitive Science 19 (4):507-562.
    Most models of computer programming explain the programmer's behaviour by a single design strategy. This article presents a cognitive architecture that uses cue‐based search to model multiple design strategies including procedural, functional, means‐end or focal, and opportunistic design. The model has been implemented in an artificial intelligence (AI) system that generates Pascal programs from English specifications.Knowledge is represented as nodes that reside in internal or external memory, where a node encodes an action that may range from a line of code (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • When is an image hallucinatory?Graham F. Reed - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-531.
  • Partial and total‐order planning: evidence from normal and prefrontally damaged populations.Mary Jo Rattermann, Lee Spector, Jordan Grafman, Harvey Levin & Harriet Harward - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (6):941-975.
    This paper examines human planning abilities, using as its inspiration planning techniques developed in artificial intelligence. AI research has shown that in certain problems partial‐order planners, which manipulate partial plans while not committing to a particular ordering of those partial plans, are more efficient than total‐order planners, which represent all partial plans as totally ordered. This research asks whether total‐order planning and/or partial‐order planning are accurate descriptions of human planning, and if different populations use different planning techniques. Using a simple (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The codes of man and beasts.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):125-136.
    Exposing the chimpanzee to language training appears to enhance the animal's ability to perform some kinds of tasks but not others. The abilities that are enhanced involve abstract judgment, as in analogical reasoning, matching proportions of physically unlike exemplars, and completing incomplete representations of action. The abilities that do not improve concern the location of items in space and the inferences one might make in attempting to obtain them. Representing items in space and making inferences about them could be done (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • The abstract code as a translation device.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):158-167.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Verbal hallucinations also occur in normals.Thomas B. Posey - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-530.
  • The uses of plans.Martha E. Pollack - 1992 - Artificial Intelligence 57 (1):43-68.
  • The representational codes for “sameness”.David S. Olton - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-154.
  • Collaborative discovery in a scientific domain.Takeshi Okada & Herbert A. Simon - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (2):109-146.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • The usefulness for memory theory of the word “store”.D. J. Murray - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):76-77.
  • Strategies in Forecasting Outcomes in Ethical Decision-Making: Identifying and Analyzing the Causes of the Problem.Michael D. Mumford, Chase E. Thiel, Jared J. Caughron, Xiaoqian Wang, Alison L. Antes & Cheryl K. Stenmark - 2010 - Ethics and Behavior 20 (2):110-127.
    This study examined the role of key causal analysis strategies in forecasting and ethical decision-making. Undergraduate participants took on the role of the key actor in several ethical problems and were asked to identify and analyze the causes, forecast potential outcomes, and make a decision about each problem. Time pressure and analytic mindset were manipulated while participants worked through these problems. The results indicated that forecast quality was associated with decision ethicality, and the identification of the critical causes of the (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.
  • Needed: Some specifics for an imaginal code.Richard Millward - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):153-154.
  • Cognition and comparative psychology.George A. Miller - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):152-153.
  • Stepwise versus globally optimal search in children and adults.Björn Meder, Jonathan D. Nelson, Matt Jones & Azzurra Ruggeri - 2019 - Cognition 191 (C):103965.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Planning routes through uncertain territory.Drew McDermott & Ernest Davis - 1984 - Artificial Intelligence 22 (2):107-156.
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement-learning perspective.Andrew C. Barto Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • What Human Planning Can Tell Us About Animal Planning: An Empirical Case.Gema Martin-Ordas - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory and mood.Maryanne Martin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-75.
  • Intentionality and autonomy of verbal imagery in altered states of consciousness.David F. Marks - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):529-530.
  • A code by any other name ….Marc Marschark - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):151-152.
  • The homunculus as bureaucrat.Alan K. Mackworth - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):74-74.
  • Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.
  • Why it is unsurprising that ape “language training” enhances “completing incomplete (external) representations of action”.Justin Leiber - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):151-151.
  • Lexical access and discourse planning: Bottom-up interference or top-down control troubles?Wendy G. Lehnert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):528-529.
  • Building machines that learn and think like people.Brenden M. Lake, Tomer D. Ullman, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Samuel J. Gershman - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Recent progress in artificial intelligence has renewed interest in building systems that learn and think like people. Many advances have come from using deep neural networks trained end-to-end in tasks such as object recognition, video games, and board games, achieving performance that equals or even beats that of humans in some respects. Despite their biological inspiration and performance achievements, these systems differ from human intelligence in crucial ways. We review progress in cognitive science suggesting that truly human-like learning and thinking (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  • Critical Decisions under Uncertainty: Representation and Structure.Benjamin Kuipers, Alan J. Moskowitz & Jerome P. Kassirer - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (2):177-210.
    How do people make difficult decisions in situations involving substantial risk and uncertainty? In this study, we presented a difficult medical decision to three expert physicians in a combined “thinking aloud” and “cross examination” experiment. Verbatim transcripts were analyzed using script analysis to observe the process of constructing and making the decision, and using referring phrase analysis to determine the representation of knowledge of likelihoods. These analyses are compared with a formal decision analysis of the same problem to highlight similarities (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations