Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Mental imagery: In search of a theory.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):157-182.
    It is generally accepted that there is something special about reasoning by using mental images. The question of how it is special, however, has never been satisfactorily spelled out, despite more than thirty years of research in the post-behaviorist tradition. This article considers some of the general motivation for the assumption that entertaining mental images involves inspecting a picture-like object. It sets out a distinction between phenomena attributable to the nature of mind to what is called the cognitive architecture, and (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   114 citations  
  • The mystery-mastery-imagery complex.H. T. A. Whiting & R. P. Ingvaldsen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):228-229.
  • Potential disparities between imagining and preparing motor skills.Charles B. Walter & Stephan P. Swinnen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):227-228.
  • Imagery needs preparation too.Stefan Vogt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):226-227.
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
  • Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content.Nigel J. T. Thomas - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (2):207-245.
    Can theories of mental imagery, conscious mental contents, developed within cognitive science throw light on the obscure (but culturally very significant) concept of imagination? Three extant views of mental imagery are considered: quasi‐pictorial, description, and perceptual activity theories. The first two face serious theoretical and empirical difficulties. The third is (for historically contingent reasons) little known, theoretically underdeveloped, and empirically untried, but has real explanatory potential. It rejects the “traditional” symbolic computational view of mental contents, but is compatible with recentsituated (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   54 citations  
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
  • Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action.J. A. Stevens - 2005 - Cognition 95 (3):329-350.
  • Bodily awareness, imagination, and the self.Joel Smith - 2006 - European Journal of Philosophy 14 (1):49-68.
    Common wisdom tells us that we have five senses: sight, hearing, smell, taste and touch. These senses provide us with a means of gaining information concerning objects in the world around us, including our own bodies. But in addition to these five senses, each of us is aware of our own body in way in which we are aware of no other thing. These ways include our awareness of the position, orientation, movement, and size of our limbs (proprioception and kinaesthesia), (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • How do we satisfy our goals?Paul G. Skokowski - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-224.
  • Canonical representations and constructive praxis: Some developmental and linguistic considerations.Chris Sinha - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):223-224.
  • Neurophysiology of preparation, movement and imagery.Jerome N. Sanes - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):221-223.
  • The ability to recall scenes is a stable individual difference: Evidence from autobiographical remembering.David C. Rubin - 2020 - Cognition 197 (C):104164.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Kinaesthetic illusions as tools in understanding motor imagery.J. P. Roll, J. C. Gilhodes & R. Roll - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-221.
  • Nonconscious motor images.Giacomo Rizzolatti - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):220-220.
  • To dream is not to (intend to) do.Jean Requin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-219.
  • Motor images are action plans.Wolfgang Prinz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-218.
  • Representations of movement and representations in movement.Giuseppe Pellizzer & Apostolos P. Georgopoulos - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):216-217.
  • Jeannerod's representing brain: Image or illusion?Jean Pailhous & Mireille Bonnard - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-216.
  • Motor simulation.Adam Morton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-215.
  • Are motor images based on kinestheticvisual matching?Robert W. Mitchell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):214-215.
  • The Understanding of Visual Metaphors by the Congenitally Blind.Ricardo A. Minervino, Alejandra Martín, L. Micaela Tavernini & Máximo Trench - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visually guided action and the “need to know”.A. David Milner, David P. Carey & Monika Harvey - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):213-214.
  • Spatial and movement-based heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch.Susan J. Lederman, Roberta L. Klatzky & Paul O. Barber - 1985 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 114 (1):33-49.
  • “To see or not to see: that is the question.” The “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” model: evidence from congenital blindness and visuo-cognitive aberrations.Steffen Landgraf & Michael Osterheider - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-213.
    Jeannerod's target article describes support, through empirical and neurological findings, for the intriguing idea of motor imagery, a form of representation hypothesized to have levels of functional equivalence with motor preparation, while being consciously accessible. Jeannerod suggests that the subjectively accessible content of motor imagery allows it to be distinguished from motor preparation, which is unconscious. Motor imagery is distinguished from visual imagery in terms of content. Motor images are kinesthetic in nature; they are parametrized by variables such as force (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The experience of spatiality for congenitally blind people: A phenomenological-psychological study. [REVIEW]Gunnar Karlsson - 1996 - Human Studies 19 (3):303 - 330.
    This phenomenological-psychological study aims at discovering the essential constituents involved in congenitally blind people's spatial experiences. Nine congenitally blind persons took part in this study. The data were made up of half structured (thorough) interviews. The analysis of the data yielded the following three comprehension forms of spatiality; (i) Comprehension in terms of image-experience; (ii) Comprehension in terms of notions; (iii) Comprehension in terms of knowledge.Comprehension in terms of image experience is the form which is most concretely and clearly experienced. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Synergy versus schema.P. C. Kainen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-212.
  • The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.Marc Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):187-202.
    This paper concerns how motor actions are neurally represented and coded. Action planning and motor preparation can be studied using a specific type of representational activity, motor imagery. A close functional equivalence between motor imagery and motor preparation is suggested by the positive effects of imagining movements on motor learning, the similarity between the neural structures involved, and the similar physiological correlates observed in both imaging and preparing. The content of motor representations can be inferred from motor images at a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   340 citations  
  • Motor representations and reality.M. Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):229-245.
  • What is coded in parietal representations?Ray Jackendoff & Barbara Landau - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):211-212.
  • Motor memory – a memory of the future.David H. Ingvar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-211.
  • Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery and mental practice.Mark Hallett, Jordan Fieldman, Leonardo G. Cohen, Norihiro Sadato & Alvaro Pascual-Leone - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-210.
  • Motor models as steps to higher cognition.Rick Grush - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):209-210.
  • Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning.Raymond W. Gibbs & Jennifer E. O'Brien - 1990 - Cognition 36 (1):35-68.
  • Peripheral and central correlates of attempted voluntary movements.S. C. Gandevia - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):208-209.
  • Call it what it is: Motor memory.Joaquin M. Fuster - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):208-208.
  • Is visual imagery really visual: Some overlooked evidence from neuropsychology.Martha J. Farah - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (3):307-17.
  • Visual-spatial movement goals.Digby Elliott & Brian K. V. Maraj - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):207-207.
  • Moving beyond imagination.Robert Dufour, Martin H. Fischer & David A. Rosenbaum - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-207.
  • Visual imagery as the simulation of vision.Gregory Currie - 1995 - Mind and Language 10 (1-2):25-44.
    Simulation Theory says we need not rely exclusively on prepositional knowledge of other minds in order to explain the actions of others. Seeking to know what you will do, I imagine myself in your situation, and see what decision I come up with. I argue that this conception of simulation naturally generalizes: various bits of our mental machine can be run‘off‐line’, fulfilling functions other than those they were made for. In particular, I suggest that visual imagery results when the visual (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Temporal representation in the control of movement.Daniel M. Corcos - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-206.
    Theories of the representation of specific kinetic and spatiotem-poral features of movement range from the explicit assertion that temporal aspects of movement are not represented to the idea that they are represented and that they have neurophysiological correlates. Jeannerod's thesis is that mental and visual images have common mechanisms and that there is a link between the image to move and the mechanisms involved with movement. The target article takes the position that certain parameters are coded in motor representations but (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The creative brain: Symmetry breaking in motor imagery.José L. Contreras-Vidal, Jean P. Banquet, Jany Brebion & Mark J. Smith - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):204-205.
  • Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.
  • On the limitations of imaging imagining.Christopher A. Buneo & Martha Flanders - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-203.
  • Visual imagery versus visual experience of familiar individuals.David J. Bryant - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (1):41-44.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do object affordances represent the functionality of an object?Ruzena Bajcsy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-202.
  • Mental imagery.Nigel J. T. Thomas - 2001 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Mental imagery (varieties of which are sometimes colloquially refered to as “visualizing,” “seeing in the mind's eye,” “hearing in the head,” “imagining the feel of,” etc.) is quasi-perceptual experience; it resembles perceptual experience, but occurs in the absence of the appropriate external stimuli. It is also generally understood to bear intentionality (i.e., mental images are always images of something or other), and thereby to function as a form of mental representation. Traditionally, visual mental imagery, the most discussed variety, was thought (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • S eeingand visualizing: I T' S n otwhaty ou T hink.Zenon Pylyshyn - unknown
    6. Seeing With the Mind’s Eye 1: The Puzzle of Mental Imagery .................................................6-1 6.1 What is the puzzle about mental imagery?..............................................................................6-1 6.2 Content, form and substance of representations ......................................................................6-6 6.3 What is responsible for the pattern of results obtained in imagery studies?.................................6-8..
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations