Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Deliberating machines.W. D. Joske - 1972 - Philosophical Papers 1 (October):57-66.
  • Needs and Rights.L. Duane Willard - 1987 - Dialogue 26 (1):43.
  • Temporalizing ontology: a case for pragmatic emergence.Ludger van Dijk - 2020 - Synthese 198 (9):9021-9034.
    Despite an attempt to break with the hierarchical picture in traditional emergentist thought, non-standard accounts of emergence are often still committed to a premise that ontology is prior to epistemology. This paper aims to topple this last remnant of the traditional hierarchy by explicating a pragmatic view of emergence based on John Dewey’s work. Dewey argued that the traditional notion of ontology is premised on a view of existence as complete. Through a discussion of Dewey’s work it is argued that (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The idea of an ethically committed social science.Leonidas Tsilipakos - 2022 - History of the Human Sciences 35 (2):144-166.
    This article presents a long overdue analysis of the idea of an ethically committed social science, which, after the demise of positivism and the deeming of moral neutrality as impossible, has come to dominate the self-understanding of many contemporary sociological approaches. Once adequately specified, however, the idea is shown to be ethically questionable in that it works against the moral commitments constitutive of academic life. The argument is conducted with resources from the work of Peter Winch, thus establishing its continuing (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Snapshots 'sub specie aeternitatis': Sinunel, Goffman and formal sociology. [REVIEW]Gregory W. H. Smith - 1989 - Human Studies 12 (1-2):19 - 57.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • On foundations research in the social sciences.Robert Boyd Skipper & Michael R. Hyman - 1995 - International Journal of Applied Philosophy 10 (1):23--38.
  • A structuralist response to a skeptic.Irving E. Sigel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):148-149.
  • Fields and the Intelligibility of Contact Action.David Sherry - 2015 - Philosophy 90 (3):457-477.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • That We Obey Rules Blindly Does Not Mean that We Are Blindly Subservient to Rules.Wes Sharrock & Alex Dennis - 2008 - Theory, Culture and Society 25 (2):33-50.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The Explanation of Action in History.Constantine Sandis - 2006 - Essays in Philosophy 7 (2):12.
    This paper focuses on two conflations which frequently appear within the philosophy of history and other fields concerned with action explanation. The first of these, which I call the Conflating View of Reasons, states that the reasons for which we perform actions are reasons why (those events which are) our actions occur. The second, more general conflation, which I call the Conflating View of Action Explanation, states that whatever explains why an agent performed a certain action explains why (that event (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Explanation in the Social Sciences: Singular Explanation and the Social Sciences.David-Hillel Ruben - 1990 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 27:95-117.
    Are explanations in the social sciences fundamentally different from explanations in the natural sciences? Many philosophers think that they are, and I call such philosophers ‘difference theorists’. Many difference theorists locate that difference in the alleged fact that only in the natural sciences does explanation essentially include laws.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Explanation in the Social Sciences: Singular Explanation and the Social Sciences.David-Hillel Ruben - 1990 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 27:95-117.
    Are explanations in the social sciences fundamentally different from explanations in the natural sciences? Many philosophers think that they are, and I call such philosophers ‘difference theorists’. Many difference theorists locate that difference in the alleged fact that only in the natural sciences does explanation essentially include laws.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Interpretation of stage as structure.Paul A. Roodin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):148-148.
  • The Befuddled Hedgehog.Richard Wood - 1987 - Philosophical Investigations 10 (3):173-199.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Theoretical and methodological problems in cross-cultural psychology.Carl Ratner & Lumei Hui - 2003 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 33 (1):67–94.
    Although cross-cultural psychology has advanced our understanding of cultural aspects of psychology, it is marred by theoretical and methodological flaws. These flaws include misunderstanding cultural issues and the manner in which they bear on psychology; obscuring the relation between biology, culture, and psychology; inadequately defining and measuring cultural factors and psychological phenomena; erroneously analysing data and drawing faulty conclusions about the cultural character of psychology. This article identifies fundamental theoretical and methodological errors that have appeared in prominent cross-cultural psychological research. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Task Structure Versus Cognitive Structure.Robert H. Pollack - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):147-148.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Winch's pluralist tree and the roots of relativism.Patrick J. J. Phillips - 1997 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 27 (1):83-95.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The linguistic thought of Ernest Gellner.Jon Orman - 2017 - Social Epistemology 31 (4):387-399.
    Theoretical questions concerning language and communication figure prominently throughout the work of the Czech-British social philosopher and anthropologist Ernest Gellner. The article traces the development of Gellner’s linguistic thought from his early, controversial engagements with Ordinary Language Philosophy to his responses to Chomsky’s work in linguistics and his late-career assessments of Wittgenstein and particularly Malinowski whose – subsequently repudiated – view of the fundamental difference between the alleged “primitive” and “scientific” functions of language turns out to play a central explanatory (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On stages and stage-building.Keith E. Nelson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):146-147.
  • Horizontal structure and the concept of stage.David Moshman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):145-146.
  • Some myths about 'mental illness'.Michael S. Moore - 1975 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 18 (3):233 – 265.
    Radical psychiatrists and others assert that mental illness is a myth. The opening and closing portions of the paper deal with the impact such argument has had in law and psychiatry. The body of the paper discusses the five versions of the myth argument prevalent in radical psychiatry: (A) that there is no such thing as mental illness; (B) that those called ?mentally ill? are really as rational as everyone else, only with different aims; that the only reasons anyone ever (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Getting the science right is the problem, not the solution: A matter of priority.Don Mixon - 1990 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 20 (2):97–110.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • From up here they look like ants1.Michael E. Malone - 1986 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 29 (1-4):407-422.
    Edward O. Wilson's sociobiology, as advocated in On Human Nature, is often criticized for its sensationalism and its anthropomorphisms. Critics have not recognized, however, that the so?called anthropomorphisms are essential to sociobiology, in so far as it seeks to address the humanities, and that the sensationalism derives from them. They are not just the sloppiness usually found in popularizations. Section I reviews the grounds for these criticisms and then ends by demonstrating that it is no less a confusion to label (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Social science, epistemology, and the problem of relativism.Volker Meja & Nico Stehr - 1988 - Social Epistemology 2 (3):263 – 271.
  • Rational choice and public affairs.Tibor R. Machan - 1980 - Theory and Decision 12 (3):229-258.
  • Cognitive activities without cognition? ethnomethodological investigations of selected ‘cognitive’ topics.Michael Lynch - 2006 - Discourse Studies 8 (1):95-104.
    Ethnomethodology and conversation analysis investigate many of the activities that are featured in the cognitive sciences. These include memory, learning, perception, and calculative activities. However, for ethno/ca such activities are not necessarily ‘cognitive’, and their investigation as activities does not necessarily require observation or speculation about what goes on within the mind or brain. This article briefly discusses three examples of nominal ‘cognitive’ activities: looking-for/seeing; failing to recall; and counting things and people. The discussion suggests how these examples can be (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The stage concept in developmental theory: a dialectic alternative.Richard M. Lerner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):144-145.
  • Psychiatry, language and freedom.Ronald Leifer - 1982 - Metamedicine 3 (3):397-416.
  • Psychiatry, language and freedom.Ronald Leifer - 1982 - Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics 3 (3):397-416.
    For political reasons, the social control functions of psychiatry are not openly recognized as such but are disguised as benevolent medical treatment. The roots of this disguise may be traced to the political revolutions in which the rule of man was replaced by the rule of law. This transformation generated a conflict between the desire for freedom under law and the desire for a greater degree of social control than is provided by law. Involuntary mental hospitalization is the neurotic resolution (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Human understanding: a question of description.Joseph T. Lawton - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):144-144.
  • Environmental factors and the organization of developmental changes.Barbara Koslowski - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):143-144.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Philosophie analytique de l'action et fondement normatif des sciences de l'homme.J. Nicolas Kaufmann - 1984 - Dialogue 23 (1):3-35.
    La philosophie analytique de l'action se réclame du langage ordinaire de l'action comme une des sources de ses data philosophiques. Elle se propose d'en examiner le fonctionnement, d'en extraire les concepts clés, de caractériser les formes de propositions dans lesquelles s'expriment nos actions et notre façon spontanée de les comprendre, d'examiner l'articulation propre aux stratégies d'action et au discours qui les justifie, et de faire des « proposals » pour la construction d'une théorie de l'action. En somme, il s'agit d'ériger (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Language, moral order and political praxis.Lena Jayyusi - 1995 - Argumentation 9 (1):75-93.
    The paper argues that the debate between objectivist criticism and postmodern critique represents a fracturing of the modes of mundane social and linguistic practice. The two together miss the open-textured character of language-in-use and the reflexive properties of situated human practice. Both difference and agreement are grounded in the multiplicity of criteria that are a feature of the logical grammar of language, and therefore of everyday praxis, including that of critique. To escape the duality of foundationalism on the one hand, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brainerd on the cognitive structure and integration criteria.Frank H. Hooper - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):142-143.
  • From epistemology to ontology: Gadamer's hermeneutics and Wittgensteinian social science. [REVIEW]Susan Hekman - 1983 - Human Studies 6 (1):205 - 224.
  • Non-indexical action.James Heap - 1975 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 5 (3):393-409.
  • The inadvertent rediscovery of self in social psychology.Susan Hales - 1985 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 15 (3):237–282.
  • Scaling, uniqueness, and integration.John W. Gyr - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-142.
  • Scientific psychology and hermeneutical psychology: Causal explanation and the meaning of human action. [REVIEW]John D. Greenwood - 1987 - Human Studies 10 (2):171 - 204.
  • Mathematical relativism: Logic, grammar, and arithmetic in cultural comparison.Christian Greiffenhagen & Wes Sharrock - 2006 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 36 (2):97–117.
  • Agency, causality, and meaning.John D. Greenwood - 1988 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 18 (1):95–115.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • On preserving and extending Piaget's contributions.Howard Gardner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-141.
  • Equilibration – the central concept of Piaget's theory.Jeanette McCarthy Gallagher - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-141.
  • Human behavior in deductive social theory: The example of economics.Robert G. Fabian - 1972 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 15 (1-4):411 – 433.
    Economists, in stressing the prescriptive implications of their analysis, typically have ignored the potential contributions of their theorems and methodological principles to the understanding of human behavior as an end in itself. The purpose of the paper is to establish the principle, by detailed reference to the literature of economics, that the 'deductive pattern of explanation' constitutes a valid approach to the general study of human behavior. As such, it is a potentially useful method of analysis in the other social (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Aesthetic Appraisal.Evan Simpson - 1975 - Philosophy 50 (192):189 - 204.
    In the twenty-five years since philosophers began to bemoan ‘the dreariness of aesthetics’, students in Wittgenstein's wake have done a great deal to eliminate the grounds of the complaint. Unfruitful essentialist theories have been largely displaced by the vigorous, if somewhat uncontrolled, growth of an enterprise which attempts to characterize and explicate aesthetic phenomena outside the desert of definition. The resulting view portrays typically aesthetic concepts as being indivisibly characterizing and evaluative, relativistic in application, necessarily linked to human attitudes, irreducible (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Symmetry Physics and Information Theory.Ernest H. Hutten - 1970 - Diogenes 18 (72):1-21.
  • Critical notice on Erwin and Fisher.Morris N. Eagle - 1983 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 13 (3):335-346.
  • The stage heuristic in the study of sensorimotor intelligence.Edward H. Cornell - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):140-141.
  • A primatological prespective.Suzanne Chevalier-Skolnikoff - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):139-140.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Against `Distributed Cognition'.Graham Button - 2008 - Theory, Culture and Society 25 (2):87-104.