Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Multiple representations of space underlying behavior.Israel Lieblich & Michael A. Arbib - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):627-640.
  • The squishy revisited: A call for ethological affirmative action.Janet L. Leonard & Ken Lukowiak - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):394-394.
  • The gap from sensation to cognition.Michael S. Landy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):101-102.
  • Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • They are really complex when you get to know them.Irving Kupfermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):393-394.
  • The cognitive map overlaps the environmental frame, the situation, and the real-world formulary.Benjamin Kuipers - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):298-299.
  • The cognitive map must be a separate module.Benjamin Kuipers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):645-646.
  • Connectionism: There's something to it.Stephen M. Kosslyn, Scott D. Mainwaring & Thomas A. Corcoran - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):297-298.
  • The role of dorsal/ventral processing dissociation in the economy of the primate brain.Marcel Kinsbourne & Charles J. Duffy - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):553-554.
  • A short quiz for neuropsychologists.Daniel P. Kimble - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):125-126.
  • Exploratory behavior without novelty drive?Arthur I. Karshmer, Derek Partridge & Victor Johnson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):644-645.
  • Lost in Chelm: Maladaptive behavior in an adaptive model.Stephen Kaplan - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):643-644.
  • Some limited neural and behavioral comparisons of the superior colliculus and the hippocampus.Walter Isaac - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):125-125.
  • Vertebrate neuroethology: Doomed from the start?David J. Ingle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):392-393.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology, according to Hoyle.Franz Huber - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):391-392.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scope of neuroethology.Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):367.
  • Neuroethology: To be, or not to be?Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):403-412.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Grandmother networks” and computational economy.J. J. Hopfield - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-100.
  • Human spatial learning.Kristina Hooper - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):642-643.
  • Invariant and programmable neuropsychological systems are fibrations.William C. Hoffman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):99-100.
  • Three frames suffice.Geoffrey E. Hinton - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):296-297.
  • Scene-based and viewer-centered representations for comparing shapes.G. Hinton - 1988 - Cognition 30 (1):1-35.
  • Ethology has progressed.Robert A. Hinde - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):391-391.
  • Attention to near and far space: The third dichotomy.Kenneth M. Heilman, Dawn Bowers & Paul Shelton - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):552-553.
  • Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Does the brain compute?Erich Harth - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):98-99.
  • Three frames suffice: Drop the retinotopic frame.Ralph Norman Haber - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):295-296.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can the aims of neuroethology be selective, while avoiding exclusivity?D. M. Guthrie - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):390-391.
  • Neuroethology and theoretical neurobiology.Stephen Grossberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):388-390.
  • Four frames do not suffice.Stephen Grossberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):294-295.
  • Brain metaphors, theories, and facts.Stephen Grossberg - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):97-98.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • More to hippocampal-collicular relations than meets the eye.Ernest Greene - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):124-125.
  • Pigeons, primates, and division of labor in the vertebrate visual system.M. A. Goodale & J. A. Graves - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):551-552.
  • Linking features in dimensions of mind and brain.Robert B. Glassman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):293-294.
  • Cortical architectures and value unit encoding.Charles D. Gilbert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):96-97.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dependence of the hippocampal formation on other brain structures.Lauren Gerbrandt - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):123-124.
  • Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.
    This target article draws together two groups of experimental studies on the control of human movement through peripheral feedback and centrally generated signals of motor commands. First, during natural movement, feedback from muscle, joint, and cutaneous afferents changes; in human subjects these changes have reflex and kinesthetic consequences. Recent psychophysical and microneurographic evidence suggests that joint and even cutaneous afferents may have a proprioceptive role. Second, the role of centrally generated motor commands in the control of normal movements and movements (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • Abstract solutions versus neurobiologically plausible problems.Jeffrey Foss - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):95-96.
  • Relationships between the superior colliculus and hippocampus: Neural and behavioral considerations.Nigel Foreman & Robin Stevens - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):101-119.
    Theories of superior collicular and hippocampal function have remarkable similarities. Both structures have been repeatedly implicated in spatial and attentional behaviour and in inhibitory control of locomotion. Moreover, they share certain electrophysiological properties in their single unit responses and in the synchronous appearance and disappearance of slow wave activity. Both are phylogenetically old and the colliculus projects strongly to brainstem nuclei instrumental in the generation of theta rhythm in the hippocampal EECOn the other hand, close inspection of behavioural and electrophysiological (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Collicular-hippocampal linkage: Reflections and further considerations.Nigel Foreman & Robin Stevens - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):133-151.
  • What's the connection?Leif H. Finkel & George N. Reeke - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):94-95.
  • Ecology and functional specialization: The whole is less than the sum of the parts.John M. Findlay - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):551-551.
  • Neuroethology according to Hoyle.Russell D. Fernald - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):387-388.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tunnel vision will not suffice.Jerome A. Feldman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):302-313.
  • Standards for neural modeling.Jerome A. Feldman & David Zipser - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):642-642.
  • Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space.Jerome A. Feldman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):265-289.
    This paper presents a general computational treatment of how mammals are able to deal with visual objects and environments. The model tries to cover the entire range from behavior and phenomenological experience to detailed neural encodings in crude but computationally plausible reductive steps. The problems addressed include perceptual constancies, eye movements and the stable visual world, object descriptions, perceptual generalizations, and the representation of extrapersonal space.The entire development is based on an action-oriented notion of perception. The observer is assumed to (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   207 citations  
  • Hoyle's new view of neuroethology: Limited and restrictive.J. P. Ewert - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):386-387.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modulation of tectal functions by prosencephalic loops in amphibians.J. P. Ewert & Th Finkenstädt - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):122-123.
  • Neuroethology or motorethology?Joachim Erber - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):386-386.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Disregarding vertebrates is neither useful nor necessary.Günter Ehret - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385-386.