Switch to: References

Citations of:

Confabulation and the frontal lobe system

In Henry L. I. Roediger & Fergus I. M. Craik (eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honor of Endel Tulving. Lawrence Erlbaum (1989)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):655-667.
    Starting from Marr's ideas about levels of explanation, a theory of the data structures and access processes in human memory is demonstrated on 10 tasks. Functional characteristics of human memory are captured implementation-independently. Our theory generates a multidimensional task classification subsuming existing classifications such as the distinction between tasks that are implicit versus explicit, data driven versus conceptually driven, and simple associative (two-way bindings) versus higher order (threeway bindings), providing a broad basis for new experiments. The formal language clarifies the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Marr versus Marr: On the notion of levels.Frank van der Velde, Gezinus Wolters & A. H. C. van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):681-682.
  • The hippocampus seen in the context of declarative and procedural control.Frederick Toates - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):771-772.
    Various apparently incompatible theories of hippocampal function have been proposed but integration is now needed. It is argued that the involvement of the hippocampus is most clearly seen when the animal needs to extrapolate beyond current sensory information. Such control can involve both the initiation of behaviour in the absence of appropriate sensory input and the inhibition of behaviour that might otherwise be triggered by current sensory input.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can we really dissociate the computational and algorithm-level theories of human memory?Guy Tiberghien - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):680-681.
  • Progress within the bounds of memory.Steven A. Sloman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):679-680.
  • Mnemonic Confabulation.Sarah Robins - 2020 - Topoi 39 (1):121-132.
    Clinical use of the term “confabulation” began as a reference to false memories in dementia patients. The term has remained in circulation since, which belies shifts in its definition and scope over time. “Confabulation” now describes a range of disorders, deficits, and anomalous behaviors. The increasingly wide and varied use of this term has prompted many to ask: what is confabulation? In recent years, many have offered answers to this question. As a general rule, recent accounts are accounts of broad (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Confabulation and constructive memory.Sarah K. Robins - 2019 - Synthese 196 (6):2135-2151.
    Confabulation is a symptom central to many psychiatric diagnoses and can be severely debilitating to those who exhibit the symptom. Theorists, scientists, and clinicians have an understandable interest in the nature of confabulation—pursuing ways to define, identify, treat, and perhaps even prevent this memory disorder. Appeals to confabulation as a clinical symptom rely on an account of memory’s function from which cases like the above can be contrasted. Accounting for confabulation is thus an important desideratum for any candidate theory of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Brain damage and cognitive dysfunction.Marlene Oscar-Berman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):678-679.
  • Is the representation meaningful? A measurement theoretic view.In Jae Myung - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):677-678.
  • What are the “goals” of the human memory system?David J. Murray - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):676-677.
  • Caught in a bind: Context information and episodic memory.Kevin Murnane - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):675-676.
  • Sequential processing of “items” and “relations”.Dave G. Mumby - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):770-771.
    Eichenbaum et al. (1994a) hypothesized that perceptually distinct items and the relations among them are processed sequentially by the parahippocampal region and the hippocampal formation, respectively. Predictions based solely on their model's sequential-processing feature might prove easier to disconfirm than those based on its representational features. Two such predictions are discussed: (1) double dissociations should be impossible following hippocampal vs. parahippocampal lesions, and (2) hippocampal lesions should not exacerbate impairments that follow complete parahippocampal lesions.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Recovered consciousness: A proposal for making consciousness integral to neuropsychological theories of memory in humans and nonhumans.Morris Moscovitch - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):768-770.
    Why is consciousness associated with recovery of memories that are initially dependent on the hippocampal system? A hypothesis is proposed that the medial temporal lobe/hippocampal complex (MTL/H) receives as its input only information that is consciously apprehended. By a process termed “cohesion,” the MTL/H binds into a memory trace those neural elements that mediated the conscious experience so that effectively, “consciousness” is an integral part of the memory trace. It is the phenomenological records of events (Conway 1992), integrated consciousness-content packets, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Task-specification language, or theory of human memory?Richard L. Lewis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):674-675.
  • Does a computational theory of human memory need intelligence?Sachiko Kinoshita - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):673-674.
  • Memory and social cognition.Yoshihisa Kashima - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):672-673.
  • On computational theories and multilevel, multitask models of cognition: The case of word recognition.Arthur M. Jacobs - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):670-672.
  • Beyond the Tower of Babel in human memory research: The validity and utility of specification.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):682-692.
  • The cognitive RISC machine needs complexity.Richard A. Heath - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):669-670.
  • Why do we need a computational theory of laboratory tasks?Robert L. Greene - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-669.
  • Strong and weak formal specifications.Richard M. Golden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-668.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hippocampal system: Dissociating its functional components and recombining them in the service of declarative memory.Howard Eichenbaum, Tim Otto & Neal J. Cohen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):772-776.
    Continuing commentary raised several issues concerning our proposal that the hippocampus, parahippocampal region, and cortical association areas mediate different aspects of memory function. Recent relevant findings strengthen our argument that neocortical areas and the parahippocampal region maintain persistent encodings of specific single items and that the hippocampus mediates representations of the relations among these items. The reciprocally and closely interconnected structures that compose the hippocampal memory system work interactively to support flexible memory expression that is relevant to the natural behavior (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The hunting of the hippocampal function.Wim E. Crusio - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):767-768.
    Eichenbaum et al.'s (1994a) theory suffers from a lack of ecological validation. It is not at all clear why the hypothesized faculties would have evolved and what their adaptive value would be. I argue that hippocampal function can only be understood if the animal is seen in its natural context.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hippocampus, delay neurons, and sensory heterogeneity.Michael Colombo & Charles G. Gross - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):766-767.
    We raise three issues concerning the Eichenbaum, Otto & Cohen (1994) model. (1) We argue against the strict division of labor that Eichenbaum et al. attribute to neocortical and limbic regions. (2) We raise the possibility that the anterior and posterior portions of the hippocampus may be important for different types of information processing. (3) We argue that, rather than reflecting relational processing, different neural responses to “match” and “nonmatch” trials may relate to different required spatial responses.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Inorganic memory.Thomas L. Clarke - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):667-667.
  • A Causal Theory of Mnemonic Confabulation.Sven Bernecker - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    This paper attempts to answer the question of what defines mnemonic confabulation vis-à-vis genuine memory. The two extant accounts of mnemonic confabulation as “false memory” and as ill-grounded memory are shown to be problematic, for they cannot account for the possibility of veridical confabulation, ill-grounded memory, and wellgrounded confabulation. This paper argues that the defining characteristic of mnemonic confabulation is that it lacks the appropriate causal history. In the confabulation case, there is no proper counterfactual dependence of the state of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations