Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The Temporal Context Model in Spatial Navigation and Relational Learning: Toward a Common Explanation of Medial Temporal Lobe Function Across Domains.Marc W. Howard, Mrigankka S. Fotedar, Aditya V. Datey & Michael E. Hasselmo - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (1):75-116.
  • Constructing Semantic Representations From a Gradually Changing Representation of Temporal Context.Marc W. Howard, Karthik H. Shankar & Udaya K. K. Jagadisan - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (1):48-73.
    Computational models of semantic memory exploit information about co-occurrences of words in naturally occurring text to extract information about the meaning of the words that are present in the language. Such models implicitly specify a representation of temporal context. Depending on the model, words are said to have occurred in the same context if they are presented within a moving window, within the same sentence, or within the same document. The temporal context model (TCM), which specifies a particular definition of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Factual memory?William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):241.
  • Episodic versus semantic memory: A distinction whose time has come – and gone?Douglas L. Hintzman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):240.
  • A flawed analogy?James Hendler - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):485-486.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cognitive RISC machine needs complexity.Richard A. Heath - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):669-670.
  • Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Much ado about the wrong thing.Yosef Grodzinsky - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):449-450.
  • Why do we need a computational theory of laboratory tasks?Robert L. Greene - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-669.
  • The possible futility of neuropsychology.Yonatan Goshen-Gottstein - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):448-449.
  • Strong and weak formal specifications.Richard M. Golden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-668.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambiguities in “the algorithmic level”.Alvin I. Goldman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):484-485.
  • The study of cognition and instructional design: Mutual nurturance.Robert Glaser - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):483-484.
  • From tools to theories: A heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology.Gerd Gigerenzer - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (2):254-267.
  • Discovery in Cognitive Psychology: New Tools Inspire New Theories.Gerd Gigerenzer - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):329-350.
    The ArgumentScientific tools—measurement and calculation instruments, techniques of inference—straddle the line between the context of discovery and the context of justification. In discovery, new scientific tools suggest new theoretical metaphors and concepts; and in justification, these tool-derived theoretical metaphors and concepts are morelikely to be accepted by the scientific community if the tools are already entrenched in scientific practice.Techniques of statistical inference and hypothesis testing entered American psychology first as tools in the 1940s and 1950s and then as cognitive theories (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.
    This target article draws together two groups of experimental studies on the control of human movement through peripheral feedback and centrally generated signals of motor commands. First, during natural movement, feedback from muscle, joint, and cutaneous afferents changes; in human subjects these changes have reflex and kinesthetic consequences. Recent psychophysical and microneurographic evidence suggests that joint and even cutaneous afferents may have a proprioceptive role. Second, the role of centrally generated motor commands in the control of normal movements and movements (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • Mental structure in the psychoses: The only hope for a neuropsychology of schizophrenia.Chris Frith - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):447-448.
  • The modularity of consciousness.Owen Flanagan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):446-447.
  • Implications of output-bound measures for laboratory and field research in memory.Ronald P. Fisher - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):197-197.
    Everyday memory tasks often require that researchers focus on output-bound measures of memory. As a result, nonmemorial processes (e.g., report option and grain size) may influence recall accuracy. These nonmemorial processes, usually eliminated by laboratory researchers, have the potential to explain some anomalous results and may even be useful to enhance everyday recollection.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary aspect of cognitive functions.J. -P. Ewert - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):481-483.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The scientific induction problem: A case for case studies.K. Anders Ericsson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):480-481.
  • The real-life/laboratory controversy as viewed from the cognitive neurobiology of animal learning and memory.Howard Eichenbaum - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):196-197.
    Parallel to Koriat & Goldsmith's accounting of human memory, there are two distinct approaches in animal learning. Behaviorist approaches focus on quantitative aspects of conditioned response probability, whereas cognitive and ethological approaches focus on qualitative aspects of how memory is used in real life. Moreover, in animal research these distinguishable measures of memory are dissociated in experimental amnesia.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • There is more going on in the human mind.Géry D'Ydewalle & Rudi Peeters - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):239.
  • A context noise model of episodic word recognition.Simon Dennis & Michael S. Humphreys - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (2):452-478.
  • Autonomous processing in parallel distributed processing networks.Michael R. W. Dawson & Don P. Schopflocher - 1992 - Philosophical Psychology 5 (2):199-219.
    This paper critically examines the claim that parallel distributed processing (PDP) networks are autonomous learning systems. A PDP model of a simple distributed associative memory is considered. It is shown that the 'generic' PDP architecture cannot implement the computations required by this memory system without the aid of external control. In other words, the model is not autonomous. Two specific problems are highlighted: (i) simultaneous learning and recall are not permitted to occur as would be required of an autonomous system; (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Context Noise and Item Noise Jointly Determine Recognition Memory: A Comment on Dennis and Humphreys (2001).Amy H. Criss & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (3):800-807.
  • Criterion Setting and the Dynamics of Recognition Memory.Gregory E. Cox & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2012 - Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (1):135-150.
    Models of recognition memory have traditionally struggled with the puzzle of criterion setting, a problem that is particularly acute in cases in which items for study and test are of widely varying types, with differing degrees of baseline familiarity and experience (e.g., words vs. random dot patterns). We present a dynamic model of the recognition process that addresses the criterion setting problem and produces joint predictions for choice and reaction time. In this model, recognition decisions are based not on the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A dynamic approach to recognition memory.Gregory E. Cox & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2017 - Psychological Review 124 (6):795-860.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Neuropsychology and mental structure: Where do we go from here?Nelson Cowan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):445-446.
  • Successive approximation in targeted movement: An alternative hypothesis.Paul J. Cordo & Leslie Bevan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):729-730.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What do memories correspond to?Martin A. Conway - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):195-196.
    Neither the storehouse nor the correspondence metaphor is an appropriate conceptual framework for memory research. Instead a meaning-based account of human memory is required. The correspondence metaphor is an advance over previous suggestions but entails an oversimple view of “accuracy.” Freud's account of memory may provide a more fruitful approach to memory and meaning.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The algorithm/implementation distinction.Austen Clark - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):480-480.
  • Inorganic memory.Thomas L. Clarke - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):667-667.
  • Functional principles and situated problem solving.William J. Clancey - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):479-480.
  • Holistic Representations of Internal and External Face Features are Used to Support Recognition.Jessica P. K. Chan & Jennifer D. Ryan - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The poverty of methodology.Alfonso Caramazza & Michael McCloskey - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):444-445.
  • Potential pitfalls in neuropsychological studies: The case of short-term memory.David Caplan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):443-444.
  • Neuropsychology – Exclusive or inclusive?Charles M. Butter & Bruno Laeng - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):442-443.
  • The correspondence metaphor: Prescriptive or descriptive?Darryl Bruce - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):194-195.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's abstract correspondence metaphor is unlikely to prove useful to memory science. It aims to motivate and inform the investigation of everyday memory, but that movement has prospered without it. The irrelevance of its competitor – the more concrete storehouse metaphor – as a guiding force in memory research presages a similar fate for the correspondence perspective.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How can cognitive neuropsychology be of value in understanding central processing?Gail A. Bruder - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):441-442.
  • Two kinds of models, many kinds of souls: Shallice on neuropsychology.Bruce Bridgeman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):440-441.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory, metamemory, and conditional statistics.Robert A. Bjork & Thomas D. Wickens - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):193-194.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's distinction between encoding processes and metamnemonic decision processes is theoretically and practically important, as is their methodology for separating the two. However, their accuracy measure is a conditional statistic, subject to the unfathomable selection effects that have hindered analogous measures in the past. We also find their arguments concerning basic and applied research mostly beside the point.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.
  • The alternative to the storehouse metaphor.Aaron Ben-Ze'ev - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):192-193.
    Koriat and Goldsmith clearly show the need for an alternative to the storehouse metaphor; however, the alternative metaphor they choose – the correspondence metaphor – is problematic. A more suitable one is the capacity metaphor.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory.Aaron S. Benjamin, Michael Diaz & Serena Wee - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (1):84-115.
  • On correspondence, accuracy, and truth.Ian Maynard Begg - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):191-192.
    Koriat & Goldsmith raise important questions about memory, but there is need for caution: first, if we define accuracy by output measures, there is a danger that a perfectly accurate memory can be nearly useless. Second, when we focus on correspondence, there is a danger that syntactic correspondence will be mistaken for historical truth.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The relation between reproductive and reconstructive processing of memory content.Harry P. Bahrick - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):191-191.
    Quantitative losses of memory content imply replicative processing; correspondence losses imply reconstructive processing. Research should focus on the relationship between these processes by obtaining accuracy- and quantity-based indicators of memory within the same framework. This approach will also yield information about the effects of task and individual-difference variables on loss and distortion, as well as the time course of each process.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuropsychological evidence and the semantic/episodic distinction.Alan D. Baddeley - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):238.
  • Consciousness and modularity.Bernard J. Baars - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):440-440.
  • Many levels: More than one is algorithmic.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):478-479.