Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • Categories, life, and thinking.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):269-283.
    Classifying is a fundamental operation in the acquisition of knowledge. Taxonomic theory can help students of cognition, evolutionary psychology, ethology, anatomy, and sociobiology to avoid serious mistakes, both practical and theoretical. More positively, it helps in generating hypotheses useful to a wide range of disciplines. Composite wholes, such as species and societies, are “individuals” in the logical sense, and should not be treated as if they were classes. A group of analogous features is a natural kind, but a group of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   185 citations  
  • Leaping up the phylogenetic scale in explaining anxiety: Perils and possibilities.Marvin Zuckerman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):505-506.
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
  • The heuristic value of representation.Thomas R. Zentall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-394.
  • The septo-hippocampal system and behavior: Difficulties in finding the exit.Michael L. Woodruff - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):504-504.
  • Effects of hippocampal lesions on some operant visual discrimination tasks.Michael L. Woodruff & Dennis L. Whittington - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):513-514.
  • The hippocampus and time.Gordon Winocur - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):512-513.
  • The metaphysics of individuality and its consequences for systematic biology.E. O. Wiley - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):302-303.
  • Mind the brain.Martha Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-393.
  • Behaviorism and voluntarism.O. S. Vinogradova - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):496-497.
  • Independent forebrain and brainstem controls for arousal and sleep.Jaime R. Villablanca - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):494-496.
  • An atropine-sensitive and a less atropine-sensitive system.Robert P. Vertes - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):493-494.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior: A critique of the arousal theory and a new synthesis.C. H. Vanderwolf & T. E. Robinson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):459-476.
    It is traditionally believed that cerebral activation (the presence of low voltage fast electrical activity in the neocortex and rhythmical slow activity in the hippocampus) is correlated with arousal, while deactivation (the presence of large amplitude irregular slow waves or spindles in both the neocortex and the hippocampus) is correlated with sleep or coma. However, since there are many exceptions, these generalizations have only limited validity. Activated patterns occur in normal sleep (active or paradoxical sleep) and during states of anesthesia (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   172 citations  
  • Brain-behavioral studies: The importance of staying close to the data.C. H. Vanderwolf & T. E. Robinson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):497-514.
  • Internal representations and indeterminacy: A skeptical view.William R. Uttal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):392-393.
  • Substrates of anxiety: But if the starting point is wrong?Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):503-504.
  • Looking for nodes and edges.Arnold Trehub - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):650-651.
  • Learning is critical, not implementation versus algorithm.James T. Townsend - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):497-497.
  • Connectionist models are also algorithmic.David S. Touretzky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):496-497.
  • The hippocampus seen in the context of declarative and procedural control.Frederick Toates - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):771-772.
    Various apparently incompatible theories of hippocampal function have been proposed but integration is now needed. It is argued that the involvement of the hippocampus is most clearly seen when the animal needs to extrapolate beyond current sensory information. Such control can involve both the initiation of behaviour in the absence of appropriate sensory input and the inhibition of behaviour that might otherwise be triggered by current sensory input.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal versus human minds.H. S. Terrace - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):391-392.
  • What is the algorithmic level?M. M. Taylor & R. A. Pigeau - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):495-496.
  • Modelling Animal Creativity from Uexküllian Approach—Attention, Search Image and Search Tone.Siiri Tarrikas - 2022 - Biosemiotics 15 (3):531-553.
    In this article, creativity is defined as a semiotic phenomenon, as a process in which the boundaries of habits and norms of social communication are exceeded and by which the challenges offered by the environment are solved. Here it is indicated that there is a direct link between attention and animal creativity and shown that there are at least two possibilities for creativity to work – one that needs attention and one which doesn’t. Animal creativity can work through several different (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Where does the cholinergic modulation of the EEG take place?J. C. Szerb & J. D. Dudar - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):493-493.
  • A ghost in a different guise.R. J. Sutherland, I. Q. Whishaw & B. Kolb - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):492-492.
  • EEG desynchronization is associated with cellular events that are prerequisites for active behavioral states.M. Steriade - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):489-492.
  • Applying Marr to memory.Keith Stenning - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):494-495.
  • In the beginning was the word.J. E. R. Staddon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):390-391.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Interactive instructional systems and models of human problem solving.Edward P. Stabler - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):493-494.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A trace of memory.D. Nico Spinelli - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):650-650.
  • Inferring anxiety and antianxiety effects in animals.Philippe Soubrié - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):502-503.
  • Reticular formation, brain waves, and coma.George G. Somjen - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):489-489.
  • Connectionism and implementation.Paul Smolensky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):492-493.
  • The development of theory: Logic of method or underlying processes?Charles P. Shimp - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):511-512.
  • Neocortical activation and adaptive behavior: Cholinergic influences.P. Shiromani & William Fishbein - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):488-489.
  • Historicism, behaviorism, and the conceptual status of memory representations in animals.Charles P. Shimp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):389-390.
  • Expectancy: The endogenous source of anticipatory activities, including “pseudoconditioned” responses.Patrick J. Sheafor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):387-389.
  • Ecologizing world graphs.Robert E. Shaw & Ennio Mingolla - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):648-650.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levels of research.Colleen Seifert & Donald A. Norman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):490-492.
  • Metatheory of animal behavior.Erwin M. Segal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):386-387.
  • Natural kinds.Stephen P. Schwartz - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):301-302.
  • The world represented as a hierarchy of nature may not require “species”.Stanley N. Salthe - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):300-301.
  • Species as individuals: Logical, biological, and philosophical problems.Michael Ruse - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):299-300.
  • The logic of representation.William W. Rozeboom - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):385-386.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Significance of localized rhythmic activities occurring during the waking state.A. Rougeul, J. J. Bouyer & P. Buser - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):488-488.
  • Weak versus strong claims about the algorithmic level.Paul S. Rosenbloom - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):490-490.
  • Typologies: Obstacles and opportunities in scientific change.Alexander Rosenberg - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):298-299.
  • Representations and cognition.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):394-406.
  • Premature closure of controversial issues concerning animal memory representations.William A. Roberts - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):384-385.