Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. What is Basic about Basic Emotions? Lasting Lessons from Affective Neuroscience.Jaak Panksepp & Douglas Watt - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (4):387-396.
    A cross-species affective neuroscience strategy for understanding the primary-process (basic) emotions is defended. The need for analyzing the brain and mind in terms of evolutionary stratification of functions into at least primary (instinctual), secondary (learned), and tertiary (thought-related) processes is advanced. When viewed in this context, the contentious battles between basic-emotion theorists and dimensional-constructivist approaches can be seen to be largely nonsubstantial differences among investigators working at different levels of analysis.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Don’t Give Up on Basic Emotions.Andrea Scarantino & Paul Griffiths - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (4):444-454.
    We argue that there are three coherent, nontrivial notions of basic-ness: conceptual basic-ness, biological basic-ness, and psychological basic-ness. There is considerable evidence for conceptually basic emotion categories (e.g., “anger,” “fear”). These categories do not designate biologically basic emotions, but some forms of anger, fear, and so on that are biologically basic in a sense we will specify. Finally, two notions of psychological basic-ness are distinguished, and the evidence for them is evaluated. The framework we offer acknowledges the force of some (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Emotion Knowledge, Emotion Utilization, and Emotion Regulation.Carroll E. Izard, Elizabeth M. Woodburn, Kristy J. Finlon, E. Stephanie Krauthamer-Ewing, Stacy R. Grossman & Adina Seidenfeld - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (1):44-52.
    This article suggests a way to circumvent some of the problems that follow from the lack of consensus on a definition of emotion (Izard, 2010; Kleinginna & Kleinginna, 1981) and emotion regulation (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004) by adopting a conceptual framework based on discrete emotions theory and focusing on specific emotions. Discrete emotions theories assume that neural, affective, and cognitive processes differ across specific emotions and that each emotion has particular motivational and regulatory functions. Thus, efforts at regulation should (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Consciousness, Attention, and the Motivation-Affect System.Tom Cochrane - 2023 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 30 (7):139-163.
    It is an important feature of creatures like us that our various motivations compete for control over our behaviour, including mental behaviour such as imagining and attending. In large part, this competition is adjudicated by the stimulation of affect — the intrinsically pleasant or unpleasant aspects of experience. In this paper I argue that the motivation-affect system controls a sub-type of attention called 'alerting attention' to bring various goals and stimuli to consciousness and thereby prioritize those contents for action. This (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Experimentally manipulated anger activates implicit cognitions about social hierarchy.Harrison M. Miller, Connor R. Hasty & Jon K. Maner - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    A correlational pilot study (N = 143) and an integrative data analysis of two experiments (total N = 377) provide evidence linking anger to the psychology of social hierarchy. The experiments demonstrate that the experience of anger increases the psychological accessibility of implicit cognitions related to social hierarchy: compared to participants in a control condition, participants in an anger-priming condition completed word stems with significantly more hierarchy-related words. We found little support for sex differences in the effect of anger on (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The myth of self-deception.Steffen Borge - 2003 - Southern Journal of Philosophy 41 (1):1-28.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait.Marvin Zuckerman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):413-434.
    A comparative method of studying the biological bases of personality compares human trait dimensions with likely animal models in terms of genetic determination and common biological correlates. The approach is applied to the trait of sensation seeking, which is defined on the human level by a questionnaire, reports of experience, and observations of behavior, and on the animal level by general activity, behavior in novel situations, and certain types of naturalistic behavior in animal colonies. Moderately high genetic determination has been (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior.Philippe Soubrié - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):319-335.
    Animal research suggests that central serotonergic neurons are involved in behavioral suppression, particularly anxiety-related inhibition. The hypothesis linking decreased serotonin transmission to reduced anxiety as the mechanism in the anxiolytic activity of benzodiazepines conflicts with most clinical observations. Serotonin antagonists show no marked capacity to alleviate anxiety. On the other hand, clinical signs of reduced serotonergic transmission (low 5-HIAA levels in the cerebrospinal fluid) are frequently associated with aggressiveness, suicide attempts, and increased anxiety. The target article attempts to reconcile such (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Serotonin, impulsivity, and emotionality.Marvin Zuckerman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):348-349.
  • Leaping up the phylogenetic scale in explaining anxiety: Perils and possibilities.Marvin Zuckerman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):505-506.
  • Is the distinction between primary and secondary sociopaths a matter of degree, secondary traits, or nature vs. nurture?Marvin Zuckerman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):578-579.
    Psychopathy has as its central traits socialization, sensation seeking, and impulsivity. These are combined in a supertrait: Impulsive Unsocialized Sensation Seeking (ImpUSS). Secondary types are defined by combinations of ImpUSS and neuroticism or sociability. All broad personality traits have both genetic and environmental determination, and therefore different etiologies (primary as genetic, secondary as environmental) for primary and secondary sociopathy are unlikely.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Home from a perilous journey.Marvin Zuckerman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):453-471.
  • Comment: Five Uses of Philosophy in Scientific Theories of Emotion.Peter Zachar - 2014 - Emotion Review 6 (4):324-326.
    Commentary on four articles in a special issue on “theories of emotion,” comparing the theories with respect to five conceptual contrasts. The first four contrasts are essentialism versus nonessentialism, discriminative versus integrative theories, individual versus social focus, and instrumentalism versus scientific realism. Although scientific psychologists appear to have reached consensus in favor of nonessentialism and they freely use both realist and instrumentalist interpretations, there is no consensus on the other two contrasts. The final contrast explored addresses attitudes toward the use (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
  • The septo-hippocampal system and behavior: Difficulties in finding the exit.Michael L. Woodruff - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):504-504.
  • Towards a theory of mood function.Muk Yan Wong - 2016 - Philosophical Psychology 29 (2):179-197.
    In light of Laura Sizer's and Robert Thayer's models of mood, I propose a functional theory to explain in what sense moods are adaptive. I argue that mood involves a mechanism which monitors our physical and mental energy levels in relation to the perceived energy demands of our environment, and generates corresponding cognitive biases in our reasoning style, attention, memory, thought, and creativity. The function of this mechanism is to engage us in the right task with the right amount of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Moral judgments by alleged sociopaths as a means for coping with problems of definition and identification in Mealey's model.Yuval Wolf - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):577-578.
    Problems of definition and identification in the integrated evolutionary model of sociopathy are suggested by Schoenfeld's (1974) criticism of the field of race differences in intelligence. Moral judgments by those labeled primary and secondary sociopaths may offer a way to validate the assumptions of the model.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What are sensation seekers seeking?Joachim F. Wohlwill - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):453-453.
  • Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis.Roy A. Wise - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):39-53.
  • Hypotheses of neuroleptic action: Levels of progress.Roy A. Wise - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):78-87.
  • Serotonin depletion and inhibition: Running the rat race without any brakes?David Wirtshafter & Karen E. Asin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):347-348.
  • Sociopathy within and between small groups.David Sloan Wilson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):577-577.
    If sociopathy is a biological adaptation, it probably evolved in small social groups in which individuals lacked the social mobility required for a con-man strategy to work. On the other hand, conflicts between groups may have provided a large niche for sociopathy throughout human history.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The biochemical basis of sensation-seeking behavior.Lars von Knorring - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):443-445.
  • The role of attachment in the development and prevention of sociopathy.Marinus H. Van IJzendoorn - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):576-577.
    Mealey's sociobiological model of sociopathy could profit from attachment theory, in particular, the theory and research on the basis of the Adult Attachment Interview (Main & Goldwyn 1985–1993). Findings of an adult attachment study in a forensic psychiatric setting are summarized. Three attachment-oriented strategies for families, schools, and forensic settings are proposed to help reduce or prevent secondary sociopathy and criminal recidivism.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is serotonin related to inhibition or generation and control of motor activity?C. H. Vanderwolf - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):346-347.
  • Controlling a neuron bomb.Luigi Valzelli - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):345-346.
  • Substrates of anxiety: But if the starting point is wrong?Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):503-504.
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
  • A discriminating case against anhedonia.T. N. Tombaugh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):77-78.
  • A cognitive-incentive view.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):683-684.
  • Sensation seeking: Where is the meat in the stew?Peter Suedfeld - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):452-453.
  • Zuckerman's sensation-seeking theory: A view from Eastern Europe.Jan Strelau - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):451-452.
  • Sensation seeking, orientation, and defense: Empirical and theoretical reservations.Robert M. Stelmack - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):450-451.
  • Neuroleptic-induced anhedonia: Some psychopharmacological implications.Philippe Soubrie - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):76-77.
  • Inferring anxiety and antianxiety effects in animals.Philippe Soubrié - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):502-503.
  • A lonesome Français in serotonin country.Philippe Soubrié - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):349-363.
  • Strategies for studying brain–behavior relationships.Paul R. Solomon - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):344-345.
  • Attention, dopamine, and schizophrenia.Paul R. Solomon & Andrew Crider - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):75-76.
  • Sensation seeking and the orienting reflex.E. N. Sokolov - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):450-450.
  • Modulating function of central serotonin neurons.E. N. Sokolov - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):344-344.
  • Is sociopathy a type or not? Will the “real” sociopathy please stand up?James Snyder - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):575-576.
    The validity of the classification of “primary sociopaths” as a qualitatively distinct group in the general population is questioned. Cenetic variation in the experience and expression of emotions may play a role in the development of antisocial behavior. However, research clearly documents that socialization environments powerfully modify the expression of genetic biases in a manner that increases or decreases the risk for “sociopathy.”.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The reward-effort model: An economic framework for examining the mechanism of neuroleptic action.Harry M. Sinnamon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):73-75.
  • Sensation seeking: Exploration of empty spaces or novel stimuli?Edward C. Simmel - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):449-450.
  • Mood-specific effects on appraisal and emotion judgements.Matthias Siemer - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (4):453-485.
  • Pathways to sociopathy: Twin analyses offer direction.Nancy L. Segal - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):574-575.
    Understanding the bases of complex behavioral phenotypes, such as sociopathy, is assisted by an evolutionary approach, in addition to other theoretical perspectives. Unraveling genetic and environmental factors underlying variant forms of sociopathy remains a key challenge for behavioral science investigators. Twin research methods (e.g., longitudinal analyses; twins reared apart) offer informative means of assessing novel hypotheses relevant to sociopathic behaviors.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The involvement of serotonergic mechanisms in anxiety and impulsivity in humans.Daisy Schalling - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):343-344.
  • Neuroscience projections to current debates in emotion psychology.Klaus R. Scherer - 1993 - Cognition and Emotion 7 (1):1-41.
  • The concept of sensation seeking and the structure of personality.Joseph R. Royce - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):448-449.
  • Evolution, mating effort, and crime.David C. Rowe - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):573-574.
    Unlike some psychiatric illnesses, criminal lifestyles are not reproductive dead ends and may represent frequency-dependent adaptations. Sociopaths may gain reproductively from their greater relative to nonsociopaths. This mating-effort construct should be assessed directly in future studies of sociopathy. Collaboration between biologically oriented and environmentally oriented researchers is needed to investigate the biosocial basis of sociopathy.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark