Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Bayes and Blickets: Effects of Knowledge on Causal Induction in Children and Adults.Thomas L. Griffiths, David M. Sobel, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Alison Gopnik - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (8):1407-1455.
    People are adept at inferring novel causal relations, even from only a few observations. Prior knowledge about the probability of encountering causal relations of various types and the nature of the mechanisms relating causes and effects plays a crucial role in these inferences. We test a formal account of how this knowledge can be used and acquired, based on analyzing causal induction as Bayesian inference. Five studies explored the predictions of this account with adults and 4-year-olds, using tasks in which (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Sensory predictions during action support perception of imitative reactions across suprasecond delays.Daniel Yon & Clare Press - 2018 - Cognition 173:21-27.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Topological Properties of Stimuli Influence Fear Generalization and Extinction in Humans.Liang Xu, Hongyu Su, Xiaoyuan Xie, Pei Yan, Junjiao Li & Xifu Zheng - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • The perception of odor objects in everyday life: a review on the processing of odor mixtures.Thierry Thomas-Danguin, Charlotte Sinding, Sã©Bastien Romagny, Fouzia El Mountassir, Boriana Atanasova, Elodie Le Berre, Anne-Marie Le Bon & Gã©Rard Coureaud - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
  • The scale of nature: Fitted parameters and dimensional correctness.D. W. Stephens - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-152.
  • Taste discrimination learning in preweanling rats.Mark E. Stanton & Michelle M. Nicolle - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (4):319-322.
  • Why are some dimensions integral? Testing two hypotheses through causal learning experiments.Fabián A. Soto, Gonzalo R. Quintana, Andrés M. Pérez-Acosta, Fernando P. Ponce & Edgar H. Vogel - 2015 - Cognition 143 (C):163-177.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Explaining compound generalization in associative and causal learning through rational principles of dimensional generalization.Fabian A. Soto, Samuel J. Gershman & Yael Niv - 2014 - Psychological Review 121 (3):526-558.
  • Possible worlds truth table task.Niels Skovgaard-Olsen, Peter Collins & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2023 - Cognition 238 (105507):1-24.
    In this paper, a novel experimental task is developed for testing the highly influential, but experimentally underexplored, possible worlds account of conditionals (Stalnaker, 1968; Lewis, 1973). In Experiment 1, this new task is used to test both indicative and subjunctive conditionals. For indicative conditionals, five competing truth tables are compared, including the previously untested, multi-dimensional possible worlds semantics of Bradley (2012). In Experiment 2, these results are replicated and it is shown that they cannot be accounted for by an alternative (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Practical effects of response specification.Richard L. Shull - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-150.
  • Awareness and reinforcement.Charles P. Shimp - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):149-150.
  • Episodic Memory as Re-Experiential Memory: Kantian, Developmental, and Neuroscientific Currents.James Russell - 2014 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 5 (3):391-411.
    Recent work on the early development of episodic memory in my laboratory has been fuelled by the following assumption: if episodic memory is re-experiential memory then Kant’s analysis of the spatiotemporal nature of experience should constrain and positively influence theories of episodic memory development. The idea is that re-experiential memory will “inherit” these spatiotemporal features. On the basis of this assumption, Russell and Hanna (Mind and Language 27(1):29–54, 2012) proposed that (a) the spatial element of re-experience is egocentric and (b) (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning.Terry Regier - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (6):819-865.
    Children improve at word learning during the 2nd year of life—sometimes dramatically. This fact has suggested a change in mechanism, from associative learning to a more referential form of learning. This article presents an associative exemplar-based model that accounts for the improvement without a change in mechanism. It provides a unified account of children's growing abilities to (a) learn a new word given only 1 or a few training trials (“fast mapping”); (b) acquire words that differ only slightly in phonological (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Memory and the integration of response sequences.Phil Reed - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):148-149.
  • From overt behavior to hypothetical behavior to memory: Inference in the wrong direction.Howard Rachlin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):147-148.
  • The rules versus similarity distinction.Emmanuel M. Pothos - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):1-14.
    The distinction between rules and similarity is central to our understanding of much of cognitive psychology. Two aspects of existing research have motivated the present work. First, in different cognitive psychology areas we typically see different conceptions of rules and similarity; for example, rules in language appear to be of a different kind compared to rules in categorization. Second, rules processes are typically modeled as separate from similarity ones; for example, in a learning experiment, rules and similarity influences would be (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Can the prosocial benefits of episodic simulation transfer to different people and situational contexts?Ding-Cheng Peng, Sarah Cowie, David Moreau & Donna Rose Addis - 2024 - Cognition 244 (C):105718.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Problems and pitfalls for Killeen's mathematical principles of reinforcement.Joseph J. Pear - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):146-147.
  • Extension to multiple schedules: Some surprising (and accurate) predictions.John A. Nevin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):145-146.
  • The relationship between mood state and perceived control in contingency learning: effects of individualist and collectivist values.Rachel M. Msetfi, Diana E. Kornbrot, Helena Matute & Robin A. Murphy - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:155572.
    Perceived control in contingency learning is linked to psychological wellbeing with low levels of perceived control thought to be a cause or consequence of depression and high levels of control considered to be the hallmark of mental healthiness. However, it is not clear whether this is a universal phenomenon or whether the value that people ascribe to control influences these relationships. Here we hypothesize that values affect learning about control contingencies and influence the relationship between perceived control and symptoms of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The propositional nature of human associative learning.Chris J. Mitchell, Jan De Houwer & Peter F. Lovibond - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):183-198.
    The past 50 years have seen an accumulation of evidence suggesting that associative learning depends on high-level cognitive processes that give rise to propositional knowledge. Yet, many learning theorists maintain a belief in a learning mechanism in which links between mental representations are formed automatically. We characterize and highlight the differences between the propositional and link approaches, and review the relevant empirical evidence. We conclude that learning is the consequence of propositional reasoning processes that cooperate with the unconscious processes involved (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • The Effects of Feature-Label-Order and Their Implications for Symbolic Learning.Michael Ramscar, Daniel Yarlett, Melody Dye, Katie Denny & Kirsten Thorpe - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (6):909-957.
    Symbols enable people to organize and communicate about the world. However, the ways in which symbolic knowledge is learned and then represented in the mind are poorly understood. We present a formal analysis of symbolic learning—in particular, word learning—in terms of prediction and cue competition, and we consider two possible ways in which symbols might be learned: by learning to predict a label from the features of objects and events in the world, and by learning to predict features from a (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Killeen's theory provides an answer – and a question.Mary Ann Metzger & Terje Sagvolden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):144-145.
  • How multiple causes combine: independence constraints on causal inference.Mimi Liljeholm - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  • Memories and functional response units.Kennon A. Lattal & Josele Abreu-Rodrigues - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):143-144.
  • Integration and specificity of retrieval in a memory-based model of reinforcement.Marvin D. Krank - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):142-143.
  • Rats, responses and reinforcers: Using a little psychology on our subjects.Peter R. Killeen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):157-172.
  • Mathematical principles of reinforcement.Peter R. Killeen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):105-135.
    Effective conditioning requires a correlation between the experimenter's definition of a response and an organism's, but an animal's perception of its behavior differs from ours. These experiments explore various definitions of the response, using the slopes of learning curves to infer which comes closest to the organism's definition. The resulting exponentially weighted moving average provides a model of memory that is used to ground a quantitative theory of reinforcement. The theory assumes that: incentives excite behavior and focus the excitement on (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Similarity-based and rule-based generalisation in the acquisition of attitudes via evaluative conditioning.Fabia Högden, Christoph Stahl & Christian Unkelbach - 2020 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (1):105-127.
    ABSTRACTGeneralisation in learning means that learning with one particular stimulus influences responding to other novel stimuli. Such generalisation effects have largely been overlooked within res...
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Adaptive Non‐Interventional Heuristics for Covariation Detection in Causal Induction: Model Comparison and Rational Analysis.Masasi Hattori & Mike Oaksford - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (5):765-814.
    In this article, 41 models of covariation detection from 2 × 2 contingency tables were evaluated against past data in the literature and against data from new experiments. A new model was also included based on a limiting case of the normative phi‐coefficient under an extreme rarity assumption, which has been shown to be an important factor in covariation detection (McKenzie & Mikkelsen, 2007) and data selection (Hattori, 2002; Oaksford & Chater, 1994, 2003). The results were supportive of the new (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Elemental representations of stimuli in associative learning.Justin A. Harris - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (3):584-605.
  • Reinforcement without representation.Stephen José Hanson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):141-142.
  • A Bio-Logical Theory of Animal Learning.David Guez - 2009 - Biological Theory 4 (2):148-158.
    This article provides the foundation for a new predictive theory of animal learning that is based upon a simple logical model. The knowledge of experimental subjects at a given time is described using logical equations. These logical equations are then used to predict a subject’s response when presented with a known or a previously unknown situation. This new theory suc- cessfully anticipates phenomena that existing theories predict, as well as phenomena that they cannot. It provides a theoretical account for phenomena (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Has learning been shown to be attractor modification within reinforcement modelling?Robert A. M. Gregson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):140-141.
  • Above and beyond the concrete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain.Michael Gilead, Yaacov Trope & Nira Liberman - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43:e121.
    In recent years, scientists have increasingly taken to investigate the predictive nature of cognition. We argue that prediction relies on abstraction, and thus theories of predictive cognition need an explicit theory of abstract representation. We propose such a theory of the abstract representational capacities that allow humans to transcend the “here-and-now.” Consistent with the predictive cognition literature, we suggest that the representational substrates of the mind are built as ahierarchy, ranging from the concrete to the abstract; however, we argue that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Moving beyond schedules and rate: A new trajectory?Gregory Galbicka - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):139-140.
  • What do reinforcers strengthen? The unit of selection.John W. Donahoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):138-139.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Influence of State Anxiety on Fear Discrimination and Extinction in Females.Pauline Dibbets & Elisabeth A. T. Evers - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What defines a legitimate issue for Skinnerian psychology: Philosophy or technology?Hank Davis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):137-138.
  • Two types of thought: Evidence from aphasia.Jules Davidoff - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):20-21.
    Evidence from aphasia is considered that leads to a distinction between abstract and concrete thought processes and hence for a distinction between rules and similarity. It is argued that perceptual classification is inherently a rule-following procedure and these rules are unable to be followed when a patient has difficulty with name comprehension and retrieval.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Changes in Cue Configuration Reduce the Impact of Interfering Information in a Predictive Learning Task.Carmelo P. Cubillas, Miguel A. Vadillo & Helena Matute - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analytic Causal Knowledge for Constructing Useable Empirical Causal Knowledge: Two Experiments on Pre‐schoolers.Patricia W. Cheng, Catherine M. Sandhofer & Mimi Liljeholm - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (5):e13137.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 5, May 2022.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning.Michael Waldmann (ed.) - 2017 - Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    Causal reasoning is one of our most central cognitive competencies, enabling us to adapt to our world. Causal knowledge allows us to predict future events, or diagnose the causes of observed facts. We plan actions and solve problems using knowledge about cause-effect relations. Without our ability to discover and empirically test causal theories, we would not have made progress in various empirical sciences. In the past decades, the important role of causal knowledge has been discovered in many areas of cognitive (...)
  • How do humans want causes to combine their effects? The role of analytically-defined causal invariance for generalizable causal knowledge.Jeffrey K. Bye, Pei-Jung Chuang & Patricia W. Cheng - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105303.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark