Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Problems in the Development of Cognitive Neuroscience Effective Communication between Scientific Domains.Edward Manier - 1986 - PSA Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986 (1):183-197.
    Could anything provide a philosophically convincing mark of the mental in simple organisms (Lloyd 1984)? Individual organisms’ capacities to modify behavior adaptively as a result of past encounters with the environment might mark the first step in the phylogeny of minds. The simplest examples of mental representation are likely to be found in the simplest forms of animal learning.The most scientifically rigorous test case of “bottom- up” strategies in cognitive neuroscience is provided by current studies of the cellular and molecular (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Approximations to a neuropsychological model of schizophrenia.Theo C. Manschreck & Brendan A. Maher - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):36-37.
  • Settling the stimulus-substitution issue is a prerequisite for sound nonteleological neural analysis of heart-rate deceleration conditioning.Robert B. Malmo & John J. Furedy - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):392-393.
  • Fear Conditioning and Social Groups: Statistics, Not Genetics.Tiago V. Maia - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (7):1232-1251.
    Humans display more conditioned fear when the conditioned stimulus in a fear conditioning paradigm is a picture of an individual from another race than when it is a picture of an individual from their own race (Olsson, Ebert, Banaji, & Phelps, 2005). These results have been interpreted in terms of a genetic “preparedness” to learn to fear individuals from different social groups (Ohman, 2005; Olsson et al., 2005). However, the associability of conditioned stimuli is strongly influenced by prior exposure to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The comparative psychology of intelligence.Euan M. Macphail - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):645.
  • From null hypothesis to null dogma.N. J. Mackintosh - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):689.
  • Comparative psychology: New experimental findings, not new approaches, are needed.Euan M. Macphail - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):395-398.
  • Comparing intelligences: Not easy, but not impossible.Euan M. Macphail - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):681.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Contiguity, contingency, adaptiveness, and controls.Glenda MacQueen, James MacRae & Shepard Siegel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):154-155.
  • Schizophrenia and attention: In and out of context.R. E. Lubow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):35-36.
  • A Bayesian Theory of Sequential Causal Learning and Abstract Transfer.Hongjing Lu, Randall R. Rojas, Tom Beckers & Alan L. Yuille - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (2):404-439.
    Two key research issues in the field of causal learning are how people acquire causal knowledge when observing data that are presented sequentially, and the level of abstraction at which learning takes place. Does sequential causal learning solely involve the acquisition of specific cause-effect links, or do learners also acquire knowledge about abstract causal constraints? Recent empirical studies have revealed that experience with one set of causal cues can dramatically alter subsequent learning and performance with entirely different cues, suggesting that (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Expecting shock.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):680-681.
  • The dark side of hegemony.Charles Locurto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):153-154.
  • Extending the “new hegemony” of classical conditioning.Dan Lloyd - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-153.
  • Overrepresentation of extreme events in decision making reflects rational use of cognitive resources.Falk Lieder, Thomas L. Griffiths & Ming Hsu - 2018 - Psychological Review 125 (1):1-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Language, evolution, and learning.Philip Lieberman - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):459.
  • Learning as a constraint on obligatory responding.Stephen E. G. Lea & Marie Midgley - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):459.
  • Classical conditioning beyond the laboratory.Hugh Lacey - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-152.
  • Tea With Milk? A Hierarchical Generative Framework of Sequential Event Comprehension.Gina R. Kuperberg - 2021 - Topics in Cognitive Science 13 (1):256-298.
    Inspired by, and in close relation with, the contributions of this special issue, Kuperberg elegantly links event comprehension, production, and learning. She proposes an overarching hierarchical generative framework of processing events enabling us to make sense of the world around us and to interact with it in a competent manner.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Bony argument.Irving Kupfermann - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):673.
  • Pavlovian conditioning: Providing a bridge between cognition and biology.Marvin D. Krank - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):151-151.
  • Beyond respondent conditioning.Sibylle Klosterhalfen & Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):149-150.
  • A promising new strategy for studying conditioned Immunomodulation.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):150-150.
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
  • A short quiz for neuropsychologists.Daniel P. Kimble - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):125-126.
  • Complexity at the organismic and neuronal levels.R. W. Kentridge - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-148.
  • Associative theory versus classical conditioning: Their proper relationship.E. James Kehoe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-147.
  • Associative learning and the cognitive map: Differences in intelligence as expressions of a common learning mechanism.Stephen Kaplan - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):672.
  • Species differences in intelligence: Which null hypothesis?James W. Kalat - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):671.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Acquisition and extinction in autoshaping.Sham Kakade & Peter Dayan - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (3):533-544.
  • Boiling down intelligence.Alison Jolly - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):671.
  • Misrepresenting the law of effect and ethology as its alternative.Timothy D. Johnston & Jennifer A. Sharp - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):458.
  • Logical and ecological inadequacies in Macphail's account of intelligence and learning.Timothy D. Johnston - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):669.
  • Excitatory amino acids, NMDA and sigma receptors: A role in schizophrenia?Karl L. R. Jansen & Richard L. M. Faull - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):34-35.
  • What is classical conditioning?W. J. Jacobs - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):146-146.
  • How to change Behavior?Iver H. Iversen - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):457.
  • Some limited neural and behavioral comparisons of the superior colliculus and the hippocampus.Walter Isaac - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):125-125.
  • A neuropsychology of psychosis.Loring J. Ingraham - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):34-34.
  • Observing others stay or switch – How social prediction errors are integrated into reward reversal learning.Niklas Ihssen, Thomas Mussweiler & David E. J. Linden - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):19-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychometric considerations in the evaluation of intraspecies differences in intelligence.Lloyd G. Humphreys - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):668.
  • Preparatory response hypotheses: A muddle of causal and functional analyses.Karen L. Hollis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):145-146.
  • Pretraining a compound conditioned stimulus reduces unblocking.Peter C. Holland - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (3):237-240.
  • Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: Image and action.Peter C. Holland - 1990 - Cognition 37 (1-2):105-131.
  • The mechanism of positive symptoms in schizophrenia.Ralph E. Hoffman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):33-34.
  • Animal general intelligence: An idea ahead of its time.William Hodos - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):668.
  • Feeding, forward and backward: Mostly red herrings.Philip N. Hineline - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):456.
  • Avoidance theory: Old wine, older bottles, a few new labels.Philip N. Hineline - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-680.
  • A cardinal principle for neuropsychology, with implications for schizophrenia and mania.David Hestenes - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):31-32.
  • The role of long-term memory and monitoring in schizophrenia: Multiple functions.Martin Harrow & Marshall Silverstein - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):30-31.
  • Learning From Success or Failure? – Positivity Biases Revisited.Tsutomu Harada - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations