Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • What and where is the unconditioned (or conditioned) stimulus in the conditioning model of neurosis?Marvin Zuckerman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):187-188.
  • Can arousal be pleasurable?Marvin Zuckerman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):449-449.
  • Short-latency avoidance responses.Kazimierz Zieliński - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-187.
  • The cost of an interrupted response pattern.Thomas R. Zentall - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):147-148.
  • The heuristic value of representation.Thomas R. Zentall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-394.
  • The Bottleneck Metaphor of Leadership Culture: How Shared Understandings About Leadership Develop in Groups and Impede Diversity and Effectiveness of Leaders.Muaz Özcan - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    There are two big problems related to leadership today: unequal representation and high failure rates among leaders. This conceptual paper argues that commonly shared values, assumptions, and beliefs about leadership, i.e., universal leadership culture, are the common cause of both problems. After the concepts and levels related to leadership culture were explained, we introduce a multilevel, multi-actor process model named the bottleneck metaphor of leadership culture. This metaphor describes how leadership cultures are co-constructed by multiple actors based on their involvement (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Role of the intrinsic modulatory systems in somesthesis.Tony L. Yaksh - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):315-315.
  • “Prepared fears” and the theory of conditioning.Wanda Wyrwicka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-186.
  • The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
  • A critique of Eysenck's theory of neurosis.Paul T. P. Wong - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):185-186.
  • The Eysenck and the Wolpe theories of neurosis.Joseph Wolpe - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):184-185.
  • Mind the brain.Martha Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-393.
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
  • Commitment: Beyond Rachlin's control?N. E. Wetherick - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):146-147.
    Rachlin's view of self-control is rejected on the grounds that his arguments do not establish the possibility of abstract, external, stimulus patterns and that his experiments, although they show that pigeons and human beings do sometimes choose postponed rather than immediate gratification, do not challenge the commonly held view that internal factors are involved in the former choice.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: A manifestation of Bayesian neural learning.James Christopher Westland & Manfred Kochen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-160.
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
  • Pattern proliferation in teleological behaviorism.Bruce N. Waller - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-146.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • Contextual determinants of pain reactions.Charles J. Vierck & Brian Y. Cooper - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):314-315.
  • Does introspection have a role in brain-behavior research?C. H. Vanderwolf & M. A. Goodale - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):448-448.
  • Internal representations and indeterminacy: A skeptical view.William R. Uttal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):392-393.
  • Introspection and science: The problem of standardizing emotional nomenclature.Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):447-448.
  • Classical conditioning beyond the reflex: An uneasy rebirth.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):161-179.
  • Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning is conducted and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
  • Further choices for molar theory.François Tonneau - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-145.
    The target article extends molar behaviorism in two positive ways: beyond average aggregates and beyond restricted laws of Although a molar framework based on purely overt events shows promise for advancing behavior theory, Rachlin's specific form of teleological behaviorism is in need of clarification.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The rat as hedonist – A systems approach.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):446-447.
  • A cognitive-incentive view.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):683-684.
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • Implications of recent research in conditioning for the conditioning model of neurosis.William S. Terry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):183-184.
  • Animal versus human minds.H. S. Terrace - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):391-392.
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
  • The role of discounting in global social issues.Craig Summers - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):144-144.
    The willingness to trade off large but ill-defined future consequences for immediate work characterizes social problems such as environmental sustainability. This commentary argues that important applications of behavioral models of self-control are being overlooked in the experimental literature. Tying the experimental literature to longterm health, environmental, and other risks makes the experimental work more germane, and raises new research questions for experimental modeling.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Transfer suppression of a hunger-motivated response as a function of the number of prior escape or avoidance trials.Gerald R. Stoffer & Harold Babb - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (5):471-474.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Softening the wires of human emotion.Michael Stocker - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):445-446.
  • A Computational Analysis of Aberrant Delay Discounting in Psychiatric Disorders.Giles W. Story, Michael Moutoussis & Raymond J. Dolan - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scale of nature: Fitted parameters and dimensional correctness.D. W. Stephens - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-152.
  • In the beginning was the word.J. E. R. Staddon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):390-391.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Emotional cookbooks.Robert C. Solomon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):444-445.
  • Conditioned alpha fear responses and protection from extinction.S. Soltysik - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):182-183.
  • The conditioned response: More than a knee-jerk in the ontogeny of behavior.William P. Smotherman & Scott R. Robinson - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):159-160.
  • Alternatives to radical behaviorism.Terry L. Smith - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):143-144.
    Operant psychologists are looking for alternatives to radical behaviorism. Rachlin offers teleological behaviorism, but it may pose as many difficulties as radical behaviorism. There is, however, a less drastic way to defend Rachlin's thesis of It portrays operant principles as relating distal efficient causes to behavioral effects.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Teleological behaviorism and internal control of behavior.Albert Silverstein - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):142-143.
  • On the nature of specific hard-wired brain circuits.Allan Siegel - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-444.
  • Practical effects of response specification.Richard L. Shull - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):150-150.
  • Long-term potentiation: What's learning got to do with it?Tracey J. Shors & Louis D. Matzel - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):597-614.
    Long-term potentiation (LTP) is operationally defined as a long-lasting increase in synaptic efficacy following high-frequency stimulation of afferent fibers. Since the first full description of the phenomenon in 1973, exploration of the mechanisms underlying LTP induction has been one of the most active areas of research in neuroscience. Of principal interest to those who study LTP, particularly in the mammalian hippocampus, is its presumed role in the establishment of stable memories, a role consistent with descriptions of memory formation. Other characteristics (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations