Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Mathematical principles of reinforcement.Peter R. Killeen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):105-135.
    Effective conditioning requires a correlation between the experimenter's definition of a response and an organism's, but an animal's perception of its behavior differs from ours. These experiments explore various definitions of the response, using the slopes of learning curves to infer which comes closest to the organism's definition. The resulting exponentially weighted moving average provides a model of memory that is used to ground a quantitative theory of reinforcement. The theory assumes that: incentives excite behavior and focus the excitement on (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Hippocampus and memory.Raymond P. Kesner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):509-510.
  • Exploratory behavior without novelty drive?Arthur I. Karshmer, Derek Partridge & Victor Johnson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):644-645.
  • Lost in Chelm: Maladaptive behavior in an adaptive model.Stephen Kaplan - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):643-644.
  • Active symbols and internal models: Towards a cognitive connectionism. [REVIEW]Stephen Kaplan, Mark Weaver & Robert French - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):51-71.
    In the first section of the article, we examine some recent criticisms of the connectionist enterprise: first, that connectionist models are fundamentally behaviorist in nature (and, therefore, non-cognitive), and second that connectionist models are fundamentally associationist in nature (and, therefore, cognitively weak). We argue that, for a limited class of connectionist models (feed-forward, pattern-associator models), the first criticism is unavoidable. With respect to the second criticism, we propose that connectionist modelsare fundamentally associationist but that this is appropriate for building models (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • What's on the minds of children?Carl N. Johnson - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):632.
  • Is Psychology a Cognitive Science?John Rust - 1987 - Journal of Applied Philosophy 4 (1):49-55.
    ABSTRACT Since the 1970s there has been a significant paradigm shift in psychology away from behaviourism and towards cognitive psychology. Indeed a recent survey discovered that by 1983 more than half American psychologists were describing their approaches as cognitive. However, while behaviourism has had an identifiable philosophical ‘line’ in positivism, reductionism and the hypothetico‐deductive model, such a framework has been noticeably lacking from cognitive psychology. A view of cognitive psychology based on functionalism and ideas from systems analysis is put forward.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory pain and conscious pain.Julian Jaynes - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):61-63.
  • Representations as metaphiers.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):379-380.
  • Considerations in evaluating the cognitive mapping theory of hippocampal function.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):509-509.
  • What is a cognitive map?Ray Jackendoff - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):507-509.
  • Unpacking the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal function.Lucia F. Jacobs & Françoise Schenk - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (2):285-315.
  • Some limited neural and behavioral comparisons of the superior colliculus and the hippocampus.Walter Isaac - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):125-125.
  • Hippocampal lesions and Intermittent reinforcement.Robert L. Isaacson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):507-507.
  • J. B. Watson's imagery and other mentalistic problems.Francis W. Irwin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):632.
  • The precision of content characterizations.Fabian Hundertmark - 2023 - Philosophical Psychology 36 (3):678-694.
    The contents of representations in non-human animals, human core cognition, and perception cannot precisely be characterized by sentences of a natural language. However, this fact does not stop us from giving imprecise characterizations of these contents through natural language. In this paper, I develop an account of the precision of content characterizations by appealing to possible-world semantics combined with set and measurement theory.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Comparative cognition revisited.Stewart H. Hulse - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):379-379.
  • Spatial Updating according to a Fixed Reference Direction of a Briefly Viewed Layout.Timothy P. McNamara Hui Zhang, Weimin Mou - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):419.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Picturing, signifying, and attending.Bryce Huebner - 2018 - Belgrade Philosophical Annual 1 (31):7-40.
    In this paper, I develop an empirically-driven approach to the relationship between conceptual and non-conceptual representations. I begin by clarifying Wilfrid Sellars's distinction between a non-conceptual capacity to picture significant aspects of our world, and a capacity to stabilize semantic content in the form of conceptual representations that signify those aspects of the world that are relevant to our shared practices. I argue that this distinction helps to clarify the reason why cognition must be understood as embodied and situated. Drawing (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Lost maps and memories.James A. Horel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):506-507.
  • Human spatial learning.Kristina Hooper - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):642-643.
  • Binary Theorizing Does Not Account for Action Control.Bernhard Hommel - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Waves and cells, maps and memories, space and time.J. Eric Holmes - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):505-506.
  • Subjective Experience in Explanations of Animal PTSD Behavior.Kate Nicole Hoffman - 2020 - Philosophical Topics 48 (1):155-175.
    Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder is a mental health condition in which the experience of a traumatic event causes a series of psychiatric and behavioral symptoms such as hypervigilance, insomnia, irritability, aggression, constricted affect, and self-destructive behavior. This paper investigates two case studies to argue that the experience of PTSD is not restricted to humans alone; we have good epistemic reason to hold that some animals can experience genuine PTSD, given our current and best clinical understanding of the disorder in humans. I (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • B. F. Skinner's confused philosophy of science.Laurence Hitterdale - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):630.
  • Hippocampal function: logic, logic, and more logic.Richard Hirsh & Joel Krajden - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):504-505.
  • The Acquisition of Survey Knowledge by Individuals With Down Syndrome.Zachary M. Himmelberger, Edward C. Merrill, Frances A. Conners, Beverly Roskos, Yingying Yang & Trent Robinson - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14:516353.
    Two experiments are reported that evaluated survey learning of youth with DS and typically developing children (TD) matched on Mental Age (MA). In Experiment 1, the experimenter navigated participants through a novel virtual environment along a circuitous path, beginning and ending at a target landmark (i.e., a door). Then, the participants were placed at a pre-specified location in the environment and instructed to navigate to the same door using the shortest possible path from their current location. They completed the task (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Editors' Introduction to Networks of the Mind: How Can Network Science Elucidate Our Understanding of Cognition?Thomas T. Hills & Yoed N. Kenett - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (1):189-208.
    Topics in Cognitive Science, Volume 14, Issue 1, Page 189-208, January 2022.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Spatial Representations in the Human Brain.Nora A. Herweg & Michael J. Kahana - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  • Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation.Linda Hermer & Elizabeth Spelke - 1996 - Cognition 61 (3):195-232.
  • A flawed analogy?James Hendler - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):485-486.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The spread of affective and semantic valence representations across states.Orit Heimer & Uri Hertz - 2024 - Cognition 244 (C):105714.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • I've got you under my skin.John Heil - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):629.
  • Cognition and representation.John Heil - 1980 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 58 (2):158-168.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding Differences in Wayfinding Strategies.Mary Hegarty, Chuanxiuyue He, Alexander P. Boone, Shuying Yu, Emily G. Jacobs & Elizabeth R. Chrastil - 2023 - Topics in Cognitive Science 15 (1):102-119.
    Navigating to goal locations in a known environment (wayfinding) can be accomplished by different strategies, notably by taking habitual, well-learned routes (response strategy) or by inferring novel paths, such as shortcuts, from spatial knowledge of the environment's layout (place strategy). Human and animal neuroscience studies reveal that these strategies reflect different brain systems, with response strategies relying more on activation of the striatum and place strategies associated with activation of the hippocampus. In addition to individual differences in strategy, recent behavioral (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding Differences in Wayfinding Strategies.Mary Hegarty, Chuanxiuyue He, Alexander P. Boone, Shuying Yu, Emily G. Jacobs & Elizabeth R. Chrastil - 2023 - Topics in Cognitive Science 15 (1):102-119.
    Navigating to goal locations in a known environment (wayfinding) can be accomplished by different strategies, notably by taking habitual, well-learned routes (response strategy) or by inferring novel paths, such as shortcuts, from spatial knowledge of the environment's layout (place strategy). Human and animal neuroscience studies reveal that these strategies reflect different brain systems, with response strategies relying more on activation of the striatum and place strategies associated with activation of the hippocampus. In addition to individual differences in strategy, recent behavioral (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Environmental overlap and individual encoding strategy modulate memory interference in spatial navigation.Qiliang He, Elizabeth H. Beveridge, Jon Starnes, Sarah C. Goodroe & Thackery I. Brown - 2021 - Cognition 207 (C):104508.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Behavioral evidence of thought and consciousness [G].D. O. Hebb - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):577-577.
  • Cortical areas involved in spatial function.H. Hécaen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):503-504.
  • Rhesus monkeys manipulate mental images.Thomas C. Hassett, Victoria K. Lord & Robert R. Hampton - 2022 - Cognition 228 (C):105225.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The bicameral retina at a glance.C. L. Hardin - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):405-406.
  • Is pain overt behavior?Gilbert Harman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):61-61.
  • Geometric determinants of human spatial memory.Tom Hartley, Iris Trinkler & Neil Burgess - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):39-75.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Reinforcement without representation.Stephen José Hanson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):141-142.
  • Odors Can Serve as Landmarks in Human Wayfinding.Kai Hamburger & Markus Knauff - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (11):e12798.
    Scientists have shown that many non‐human animals such as ants, dogs, or rats are very good at using smells to find their way through their environments. But are humans also capable of navigating through their environment based on olfactory cues? There is not much research on this topic, a gap that the present research seeks to bridge. We here provide one of the first empirical studies investigating the possibility of using olfactory cues as landmarks in human wayfinding. Forty subjects participated (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Leibnizian privacy and Skinnerian privacy.Keith Gunderson - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):628.
  • Introduction to the Topic on Modeling Spatial Cognition.Glenn Gunzelmann - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (4):628-631.
    Our ability to process spatial information is fundamental for understanding and interacting with the environment, and it pervades other components of cognitive functioning from language to mathematics. Moreover, technological advances have produced new capabilities that have created research opportunities and astonishing applications. In this Topic on Modeling Spatial Cognition, research crossing a variety of disciplines and methodologies is described, all focused on developing models to represent the capacities and limitations of human spatial cognition.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self, world and space: The meaning and mechanisms of ego- and allocentric spatial representation.Rick Grush - 2000 - Brain and Mind 1 (1):59-92.
    b>: The problem of how physical systems, such as brains, come to represent themselves as subjects in an objective world is addressed. I develop an account of the requirements for this ability that draws on and refines work in a philosophical tradition that runs from Kant through Peter Strawson to Gareth Evans. The basic idea is that the ability to represent oneself as a subject in a world whose existence is independent of oneself involves the ability to represent space, and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • The rat-a-gorical imperative: Moral intuition and the limits of affective learning.Joshua D. Greene - 2017 - Cognition 167 (C):66-77.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • On panspatial theories of brain and behavior.Ernest Greene - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):503-503.