Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The mechanisms of human action: introduction and background.Ezequiel Morsella - 2009 - In Ezequiel Morsella, John A. Bargh & Peter M. Gollwitzer (eds.), Oxford handbook of human action. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 1--32.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Kognitivní kontrarevoluce?Jaroslav Peregrin - 2012 - Filosofie Dnes 4 (1):19-35.
    Ve standardních výkladech moderních dějin studia mysli ve dvacátém století se dočteme, že zatímco kolem poloviny tohoto století ovládl studium mysli zpozdilý behaviorismus, v šedesátých letech nastoupila „kognitivní revoluce“, která nadvládu behaviorismu smetla a otevřela cestu ke skutečně nepředpojatému a adekvátnímu studiu mysli. V tomto textu se chci nad tímto standardním výkladem zamyslet a zpochybnit ho: konkrétně chci poukázat na to, že behaviorismus nebyl ve všech ohledech tak zpozdilý, jak by se z tohoto pohledu mohl jevit; a že „kognitivní revoluce“ (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How not to decide whether inner speech is speech: Two common mistakes.Daniel Gregory - 2024 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 23 (2):231-252.
    Philosophical interest in inner speech has grown in recent years. In seeking to understand the phenomenon, many philosophers have drawn heavily on two theories from neighbouring disciplines: Lev Vygotsky’s theory on the development of inner speech in children and a cognitive-scientific theory about speech production. I argue that they have been too uncritical in their acceptance of these theories, which has prevented a proper analysis of inner speech.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Enciclopédia de Termos Lógico-Filosóficos.João Miguel Biscaia Branquinho, Desidério Murcho & Nelson Gonçalves Gomes (eds.) - 2006 - São Paulo, SP, Brasil: Martins Fontes.
    Esta enciclopédia abrange, de uma forma introdutória mas desejavelmente rigorosa, uma diversidade de conceitos, temas, problemas, argumentos e teorias localizados numa área relativamente recente de estudos, os quais tem sido habitual qualificar como «estudos lógico-filosóficos». De uma forma apropriadamente genérica, e apesar de o território teórico abrangido ser extenso e de contornos por vezes difusos, podemos dizer que na área se investiga um conjunto de questões fundamentais acerca da natureza da linguagem, da mente, da cognição e do raciocínio humanos, bem (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • What and where is the unconditioned (or conditioned) stimulus in the conditioning model of neurosis?Marvin Zuckerman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):187-188.
  • Can arousal be pleasurable?Marvin Zuckerman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):449-449.
  • Short-latency avoidance responses.Kazimierz Zieliński - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-187.
  • The use of crying over spilled milk: A note on the rationality and functionality of regret.Marcel Zeelenberg - 1999 - Philosophical Psychology 12 (3):325 – 340.
    This article deals with the rationality and functionality of the existence of regret and its influence on decision making. First, regret is defined as a negative, cognitively based emotion that we experience when realizing or imagining that our present situation would have been better had we acted differently. Next, it is discussed whether this experience can be considered rational and it is argued that rationality only applies to what we do with our regrets, not to the experience itself. Then, research (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • “Prepared fears” and the theory of conditioning.Wanda Wyrwicka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):186-186.
  • A critique of Eysenck's theory of neurosis.Paul T. P. Wong - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):185-186.
  • The Eysenck and the Wolpe theories of neurosis.Joseph Wolpe - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):184-185.
  • A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications.S. F. Walker - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):17-38.
    Critics of the computational connectionism of the last decade suggest that it shares undesirable features with earlier empiricist or associationist approaches, and with behaviourist theories of learning. To assess the accuracy of this charge the works of earlier writers are examined for the presence of such features, and brief accounts of those found are given for Herbert Spencer, William James and the learning theorists Thorndike, Pavlov and Hull. The idea that cognition depends on associative connections among large networks of neurons (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Does introspection have a role in brain-behavior research?C. H. Vanderwolf & M. A. Goodale - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):448-448.
  • Introspection and science: The problem of standardizing emotional nomenclature.Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):447-448.
  • A behavioral theory of social structure.Jonathan H. Turner - 1988 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 18 (4):355–372.
  • The rat as hedonist – A systems approach.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):446-447.
  • Implications of recent research in conditioning for the conditioning model of neurosis.William S. Terry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):183-184.
  • Softening the wires of human emotion.Michael Stocker - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):445-446.
  • Scrutinizing the Criteria for Character Strengths: Laypersons Assert That Every Strength Is Positively Morally Valued, Even in the Absence of Tangible Outcomes.Alexander G. Stahlmann & Willibald Ruch - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Emotional cookbooks.Robert C. Solomon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):444-445.
  • Conditioned alpha fear responses and protection from extinction.S. Soltysik - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):182-183.
  • Objectivism and the study of man (part I).Hans Skjervheim - 1974 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 17 (1-4):213 – 239.
    The purpose of this study is to show that the distinctions made by Wilhelm Dilthey and Max Weber between the natural sciences and the 'Geisteswissen-schaften' are sound in principle, pace the arguments to the contrary within classical logical empiricism. It is held that intentional contexts are characteristic of social science. Intentional contexts are held to be more important in psychology than mental states, like toothache. If logical behaviourism is to have any plausibility, it has to be shown how intentional contexts (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • On the nature of specific hard-wired brain circuits.Allan Siegel - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-444.
  • The objects of action explanation.Constantine Sandis - 2012 - Ratio 25 (3):326-344.
    This paper distinguishes between various different conceptions of behaviour and action before exploring an accompanying variety of distinct things that ‘action explanation’ may plausibly amount to viz. different objectives of action explanation. I argue that a large majority of philosophers are guilty of conflating many of these, consequently offering inadequate accounts of the relation between actions and our reasons for performing them. The paper ends with the suggestion that we would do well to opt for a pluralistic understanding of action (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • History, origin myth and ideology: 'Discovery of social psychology.Franz Samelson - 1974 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 4 (2):217–232.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Modeling neurosis: one type of learning is not enough.Kurt Salzinger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):181-182.
  • On the complexity of emotion.Joseph R. Royce - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-443.
  • Thesis and antithesis: S-R levers or meaning-perceivers?Ted L. Rosenthal - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):181-181.
  • Insects, instincts and boundary work in early social psychology.Diane M. Rodgers - 2013 - History of the Human Sciences 26 (1):68-89.
    Insects factored as ‘symbols of instinct’, necessary as a rhetorical device in the boundary work of early social psychology. They were symbolically used to draw a dividing line between humans and animals, clarifying views on instinct and consciousness. These debates were also waged to determine if social psychology was a subfield of sociology or psychology. The exchange between psychologist James Mark Baldwin and sociologist Charles Abram Ellwood exemplifies this particular aspect of boundary work. After providing a general background of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Emotions at the Service of Cultural Construction.Bernard Rimé - 2019 - Emotion Review 12 (2):65-78.
    Emotions signal flaws in the person’s anticipation systems, or in other words, in aspects of models of how the world works. As these models are essentially shared in society, emotional challenges e...
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Journey into the interior of the organism.Howard Rachlin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):180-181.
  • Introduction: Thought as Language.John Preston - 1997 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 42:1-.
    Western philosophy has a long-standing interest in the relationship between thought and language. This is not least because language use and our mental capacities are so central to our human self-conception, as well as to the ways in which we have tried to think about other beings. Retrospectively, it is possible to identify certain broad traditions in the philosophical study of thought and language, traditions which also have their representatives in psychology and linguistics. In this introduction I shall focus on (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Only four command systems for all emotions?Robert Plutchik - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):442-443.
  • Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions.Jaak Panksepp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):407-422.
  • Archaeology of mind.Jaak Panksepp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):449-467.
  • Toward an unpdated model of neurosis.J. M. Notterman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-179.
  • The Role of Motor Inhibition During Covert Speech Production.Ladislas Nalborczyk, Ursula Debarnot, Marieke Longcamp, Aymeric Guillot & F.-Xavier Alario - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Covert speech is accompanied by a subjective multisensory experience with auditory and kinaesthetic components. An influential hypothesis states that these sensory percepts result from a simulation of the corresponding motor action that relies on the same internal models recruited for the control of overt speech. This simulationist view raises the question of how it is possible to imagine speech without executing it. In this perspective, we discuss the possible role played by motor inhibition during covert speech production. We suggest that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychobiology needs cognitive psychology.Adam Morton - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):441-442.
  • New perspectives on conditioning models and incubation theory.Susan Mineka - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):178-178.
  • Sex and sensibility: The role of social selection: Roughgarden, Joan: The genial gene: Deconstructing Darwinian selfishness. Berkeley: University of California Press, 2009, ix+261pp, $40.00 HB, $18.95 PB.Erika L. Milam, Roberta L. Millstein, Angela Potochnik & Joan E. Roughgarden - 2010 - Metascience 20 (2):253-277.
    Sex and sensibility: The role of social selection Content Type Journal Article DOI 10.1007/s11016-010-9464-6 Authors Erika L. Milam, Department of History, University of Maryland, 2115 Francis Scott Key Hall, College Park, MD 20742, USA Roberta L. Millstein, Department of Philosophy, University of California, Davis, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616, USA Angela Potochnik, Department of Philosophy, University of Cincinnati, P.O. Box 210374, Cincinnati, OH 45221, USA Joan E. Roughgarden, Department of Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-5020, USA Journal Metascience Online (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Are the concepts of enhancement and preparedness necessary?Wallace R. McAllister & Dorothy E. McAllister - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):177-178.
  • Conditioning models for clinical syndromes are out of date.Isaac Marks - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):175-177.
  • Reflections on the conditioning model of neurosi.Michael J. Mahoney - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):174-175.
  • On some key concepts in Eysenck's conditioning theory of neurosis.William Lyons - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):174-174.
  • Concerning the alleged four basic emotions.William Lyons - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):440-441.
  • Introspection and cultural knowledge systems.Catherine Lutz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):439-440.
  • The motor theory of speech perception revised.Alvin M. Liberman & Ignatius G. Mattingly - 1985 - Cognition 21 (1):1-36.
  • A reconsideration of Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis.Donald J. Levis - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):172-174.
  • Psychobiology without psychosocial significance.Richard S. Lazarus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):438-439.
  • Eysenck on Watson: paying lip service to lip service.Leonard Krasner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):172-172.