Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Fluid Biosemiotic Mechanisms Underlie Subconscious Habits

  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although research into the biosemiotic mechanisms underlying the purposeful behavior of brainless living systems is extensive, researchers have not adequately described biosemiosis among neurons. As the conscious use of signs is well-covered by the various fields of semiotics, we focus on subconscious sign action. Subconscious semiotic habits, both functional and dysfunctional, may be created and reinforced in the brain not necessarily in a logical manner and not necessarily through repeated reinforcement. We review literature that suggests hypnosis may be effective in changing subconscious dysfunctional habits, and we offer a biosemiotic framework for understanding these results. If it has been difficult to evaluate any psychological approach, including hypnosis, this may be because contemporary neuroscience lacks a theory of the sign. We argue that understanding the fluid nature of representation in biological organisms is prerequisite to understanding the nature of the subconscious and may lead to more effective of treatments for dysfunctional habits developed through personal experience or culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In addition to the term “semiotic habit,” which can only be applied to the actions and responses of living or purposeful entities, we also use various less restricted and less precise synonyms: procedure, program, routine, algorithm and machine—in order to engage disciplines that use these more familiar terms.

  2. Elsewhere (Alexander 2009: 97; Alexander 2011: 30, 114) makes a distinction between different kinds of selection: statistical selection for most numerous, which gives us average regularity and homogeneity as well as, ultimately, the Second Law of Thermodynamics; formal selection for similarity and proximity, which give us self-organized patterns; and functional selection for reproductive fitness, which gives us evolution. Here we refer to formal selection as “self-organization” to avoid confusion with the other types of selection.

  3. As Prigogine and Nicolis (1977), Salthe (1993), Schneider and Sagan (2005), Deacon (2011) and others have noted, complex adaptive systems (CAS) reduce gradients more efficiently than strictly mechanical systems.

  4. Computer scientists are in the experimental stages of developing reaction-diffusion computing systems that self-organize virtual circuits. These may prove to be more life-like and develop true intelligence at some point. Currently however, such systems aren’t quite as smart as slime mold. These scientists have actually started using slime mold for some of their experiments. See Adamatzky et al. (2005).

  5. The illustration is not meant to accurately represent starling behavior; the purpose is to give the reader a generic idea of a computational machine.

References

  • Abel, D. (2009). The GS (genetic selection) principle. Frontiers of Bioscience, 14(4), 2959–2969.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Adamatzky, A., Costello, B., & Asai, T. (2005). Reaction-diffusion computers. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, V. (2009). The poetics of purpose. Biosemiotics, 2(1), 77–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, V. (2011). The Biologist’s mistress: Rethinking self-organization in art, literature, and nature. Litchfield Park: Emergent Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, V. (2017/2018). Siri fails the Turing test: Computation, biosemiosis and artificial life. Recherches Sémiotiques / Semiotic Inquiry. Forthcoming.

  • Arnellos, A., Bruni, L., El-Hani, C., & Collier, J. (2012). Anticipatory functions, digital-analog forms and biosemiotics: Integrating the tools to model information and normativity in autonomous biological agents. Biosemiotics, 5(3), 331–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baslow, M. (2011). Biosemiosis and the cellular basis of mind. Biosemiotics, 4(1), 39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.-J., Oakley, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Delusions of alien control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 1058–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. (1970). Analysis of a mnemonic device: Modern psychology uncovers the powerful components of an ancient system for improving memory. American Scientist, 58(5), 496–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braffman, W., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability: An empirical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 578–587.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, R. A., Hung, L., & Dobson-Stone, C. (2013). The association between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and hypnotizability. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(10), 1979–1984.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castro García, Ò. (2011). Principles of minimal cognition in smart slime molds and social bacteria. Pensamiento, 67(254), 787–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crutchfield, J. (1994). The calculi of emergence: Computation, dynamics, and induction. Physica D, 75, 11–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Assis, L. (2015). Neural binding, consciousness, and mental disorder: Complexity as a common element. Journal for Neurocognitive Research, 57(3–4), 110–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (2011). Incomplete nature: How mind emerged from matter. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derbyshire, S., Whalley, M., Stenger, V. A., & Oakley, D. (2004). Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. NeuroImage, 23(1), 392–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmeche, C., & Hoffmeyer, J. (1991). Code-duality and the semiotics of nature. In M. Anderson & F. Merrell (Eds.), On semiotic modeling (pp. 117–166). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faria, M. (2008). Signal transduction codes and cell fate. In M. Barberi (Ed.), The codes of life (pp. 265–283). Amsterdam: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Favareau, D. (2009). Essential readings in biosemiotics: Anthology and commentary. Amsterdam: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Faymondville, M.-E., Roediger, L., Del Fiore, G., Delgueldre, C., Phillips, C., Lamy, M., Maquet, P., & Laureys, S. (2003). Increased cerebral functional connectivity underlying the antinociteptive effects of hypnosis. Cognition and Brain Research, 17(2), 255–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S, & Greenberg, R. (1985). The Scientific Credibility of Freud's Theories and Therapy New York: Columbia University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. (2000). How brains make up their minds. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, C., & Sigman, M. (2007). Brain states: Top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron, 54(5), 677–696.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, F., Maggio, R., & Bruni, L. (2011). Are olfactory receptors really Olfactive? Biosemiotics, 4(3), 331–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, J. (2013). Sensory perception: Lessons from synesthesia: Using synesthesia to inform the understanding of sensory perception. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 86(2), 203–216.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horton, J., & Crawford, H. (2004). Neurophysiological and genetic determinants of high hypnotizability. (pp. 133–151). In M. Heap et al. (Eds.), The highly hypnotizable person: Theoretical, experimental and clinical issues. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J., Bressler, S. L., Buchanan, S., DeGuzman, G. C., Ding, M., Fuchs, A., & Holroydet, T. (1991). A phase transition in human brain and behavior. Physics Letters A, 169(3), 134–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S., Thompson, W., Costantini-Ferrando, M., Alpert, N., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(8), 1279–1284.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenberg, P., Bachner-Melman, R., Gritsenko, I., & Ebstein, R. (2000). Exploratory association study between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) high/low enzyme activity polymorphism and hypnotizability. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96(6), 771–774.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liossi, C., & Hatira, P. (1999). Clinical hypnosis versus cognitive behavioral training for pain management with pediatric cancer patients undergoing bone marrow aspirations. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47(2), 104–116.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meléndez-Hevia, E., Waddell, T. G., & Cascante, M. (1996). The puzzle of the Krebs citric acid cycle: Assembling the pieces of chemically feasible reactions, and opportunism in the design of metabolic pathways during evolution. Journal of Molecular Evolution, 43(3), 293–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y. (2008). The immune self code: From correspondence to complexity. In M. Barbieri (Ed.), The codes of life (pp. 247–263). Amsterdam: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Noonan, D. (2004). Altered states. In Newsweek., October 3, 2004. http://www.newsweek.com/id/55125. Accessed 6 Feb 2017.

  • Ó Nualláin, S. (2010). Ask not what you can do for yourself: Cartesian chaos, neural dynamics, and immunological cognition. Biosemiotics, 3(1), 79–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ó Nualláin, S., & Doris, T. (2012). Consciousness is cheap, even if symbols are expensive: Metabolism and the brain’s dark energy. Biosemiotics, 5(2), 193–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakley, D., & Halligan, P. (2013). Hypnotic suggestion: Opportunities for cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(8), 565–576.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, D., Miller, A., Rowlands, G., & Turner, M. (2014). Role of projection in the control of bird flocks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(29), 10422–10426 https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QPZbedm_fBA (Accessed April 14, 2017.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Postle, B. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139(1), 23–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, G. (2004). Intensive therapy: Utilizing hypnosis in the treatment of substance abuse disorders. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 47(1), 21–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, L., Brody, D., & Gu, Q. (2011). Antidepressant use in persons aged 12 and over: United States, 2005–2008. NCHS Data Brief 76. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db76.pdf. Accessed 30 Jan 2017.

  • Prigogine, I., & Nicolis, G. (1977). Self-Organization in non-Equilibrium Systems. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rainville, P., Hofbauer, R., Paus, T., & Price, D. (1999). Cerebral mechanisms of hypnotic induction and suggestion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 110–125.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., & Buhle, J. (2006). Typologies of attentional networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(5), 367–379.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., & Wolfson, J. (2010). From dynamic lesions to brain imaging of behavioral lesions: Alloying the gold of psychoanalysis with the copper of suggestion. Neuropsychoanalysis, 12(1), 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., & Posner, M. (2002). Hypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(12), 1155–1161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, C., Lattya, T., Dussutour, A., & Beekman, M. (2012). Slime mold uses an externalized spatial “memory” to navigate in complex environments. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, 109(43), 17490–17494.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. (1987). Flocks, herds and schools: A distributed behavioral model. SIGGRAPH '87: Proceedings of the 14th annual conference on Computer graphics and interactive techniques. Association for Computing Machinery: 25–34.

  • Roozendaal, B. (2002). Stress and memory: Opposing effects of glucocorticoids on memory consolidation and memory retrieval. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78(3), 578–595.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthe, S. (1993). Development and evolution: Complexity and change in biology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, E., & Sagan, D. (2005). Into the cool: Energy flow, thermodynamics, and life. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharov, A. (2016). Evolution of natural agents: Preservation, advance, and emergence of functional information. Biosemiotics, 9(1), 103–120.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szechtman, H., Woody, E., Bowers, K. S., & Nahmias, C. (1998). Where the imaginal appears real: A positron emission tomography study of auditory hallucinations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 95(4), 1956–1960.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tebbetts, C. (1977). Self-Hypnosis. Glendale: Westwood Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terhune, D. B., Cardeña, E., & Lindgren, M. (2010). Disruption of synaesthesia by posthypnotic suggestion: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), 3360–3364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind Cambridge: Harvard University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A. (1952). The chemical basis of morphogeneis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 237(641), 37–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uhlhaas, P., Pipa, G., Lima, B., Melloni, L., Neuenschwander, S., Nikolic, D., & Singer, W. (2009). Neural synchrony in cortical networks: History, concept and current status. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 3(17), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vermetten, E., & Bremmer, J. D. (2004). Functional brain imagine and the induction of traumatic recall: A cross-correlational review between neuroimaging and hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 52(3), 280–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. (2002). The illusion of conscience will. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winfree, A. (1987). When time breaks down: The three-dimensional dynamics of electrochemical waves and cardiac arrhythmias. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to V. N. Alexander.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Alexander, V.N., Grimes, V. Fluid Biosemiotic Mechanisms Underlie Subconscious Habits. Biosemiotics 10, 337–353 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-017-9298-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-017-9298-3

Keywords

Navigation