Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 November 2020
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.593010

Negative Bias During Early Attentional Engagement in Major Depressive Disorder as Examined Using a Two-Stage Model: High Sensitivity to Sad but Bluntness to Happy Cues

Xiang Ao1,2† Licheng Mo2† Zhaoguo Wei3 Wenwen Yu2 Fang Zhou2 Dandan Zhang1,2,4,5*
  • 1Institute of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 3Department of Clinical Psychology, Shenzhen Kangning Hospital, Shenzhen, China
  • 4Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China
  • 5Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen, China

Negative attentional bias has been well established in depression. However, there is very limited knowledge about whether this depression-relevant negative bias exits during initial attentional allocation, as compared with the converging evidence for the negative bias during sustained attention engagement. This study used both behavioral and electrophysiological measures to examine the initial attention engagement in depressed patients influenced by mood-congruent and mood-incongruent emotions. The dot-probe task was performed with a 100-ms exposure time of the emotional cues (emotional and neutral face pairs). The behavioral results showed that the patients responded faster following valid compared with invalid sad facial cues. Electrophysiological indexes in the frame of the two-stage model of attentional modulation by emotions provided cognitive mechanisms in distinct attention engagement stages: (1) the patients exhibited reduced P1 amplitudes following validly than invalidly happy cues than did the healthy controls, indicating a positive attenuation at an early stage of automatic attention orientation; and (2) the patients exhibited enhanced whereas the controls showed reduced P3 amplitudes following validly than invalidly sad cues, suggesting a mood-congruent negative potentiation in depression at the later stage of top-down voluntary control of attention. Depressed patients show a negative bias in early attentional allocation, reflected by preferred engagement with mood-congruent and diminished engagement with positive emotional cues/stimuli.

Introduction

Major depression disorder (MDD) is a common mental disorder characterized by general and persistent depressed mood as well as loss of interest and enjoyment (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). The MDD is a major contributor to the overall global burden of disease and is responsible for more “years lost” to disability worldwide than any other diseases (Smith and De Torres, 2014; Ferrari et al., 2016). According to the cognitive model of depression (Beck, 2008; Disner et al., 2011), a negative processing bias, reflected by extraordinarily enhanced reactivity to negative emotional cues or events (i.e., negative potentiation) and a reduced reactivity in response to positive emotional stimuli (i.e., positive attenuation), contributes to the onset, development, and maintenance of the disorder (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010). Typically, the negative attentional bias is a key component of the cognitive deficits; that is, MDD patients, when compared with healthy controls, demonstrate increased vigilance and selective attention to negative stimuli including negative facial expressions and words while they pay reduced attention to positive information (Mathews et al., 1996; Le et al., 2007; Leyman et al., 2007; Kellough et al., 2008; Fritzsche et al., 2010; Peckham et al., 2010; Yiend, 2010; Armstrong and Olatunji, 2012; García-Blanco et al., 2014; for reviews see Peckham et al., 2010; Yiend, 2010; Armstrong and Olatunji, 2012; Winer and Salem, 2016). It should be noted that the negative processing bias in MDD is distinct from the “negativity bias” (Ito et al., 1998) observed in healthy people. While the negativity bias developed during evolution denotes the enhanced emotional modulation effect of threat-related cues/stimuli (compared with positive and neutral stimuli) on one’s cognitive processes (Vuilleumier et al., 2001; Pessoa et al., 2002; Vuilleumier, 2005), the depression-relevant negative bias emphasizes on mood-congruent (i.e., sad) stimuli/events (Gotlib et al., 2004b; Koster et al., 2005; Arnone et al., 2012). For example, studies have revealed that MDD patients usually show attentional bias to sad facial pictures or depression-related words (Mathews and MacLeod, 2005; Peckham et al., 2010).

The dot-probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986; Brosch et al., 2008) with emotional stimuli as cues is frequently employed to investigate attentional modulation effects of emotion (MacLeod et al., 1986; Pourtois and Vuilleumier, 2006). In this task, pairs of stimuli (e.g., a fearful face and a neutral face) are briefly presented (as the cue) and immediately followed by a dot (as the probe) in the location of one of the previous stimuli. Participants are required to respond as quickly as possible to the probe. The rationale is that responses will be facilitated if the probe occurs in the location where participants already attend (Posner et al., 1980). The dot-probe task has been acknowledged as an effective tool to detect the impaired attentional bias in depressed patients (Peckham et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2014; Kappenman et al., 2014; Winer and Salem, 2016; Zhao et al., 2017).

Using this paradigm, previous studies have demonstrated that MDD patients often exhibit an attentional bias toward negative stimuli and/or away from positive stimuli when compared with healthy individuals (e.g., Gotlib et al., 2004a; Joormann and Gotlib, 2007; Zhou et al., 2015). However, it seems that the negative attentional bias in depression detected by the dot-probe task depends on the exposure time of the cues. While converging evidence has revealed that sustained negative attentional bias could be robustly observed with 1,000-ms or longer cue presentation durations (Mogg et al., 1995; Bradley et al., 1997; Gotlib et al., 2004a; Koster et al., 2005; Joormann and Gotlib, 2007; Fritzsche et al., 2010; Hankin et al., 2010; Günther et al., 2015; for review see Mathews and MacLeod, 2005; De Raedt and Koster, 2010; Gotlib and Joormann, 2010), there is no clear evidence of this negative attentional bias in MDD patients for short cue exposure durations (e.g., 100 ms), especially relevant for early attention engagement processes (Neshat-Doost et al., 2000; Donaldson et al., 2007). It has been proposed that the cue exposure time of 1,000 ms or longer allows more than one attention shift, and the negative bias found in this case indicates that depressed individuals show difficulties in disengaging attention from negative stimuli (Bradley et al., 1997; Gotlib et al., 2004a). To examine the negative bias reflecting an initial orienting toward negative stimuli, a 100-ms presentation time is suggested as the cue exposure time (Cooper and Langton, 2006). Considering the critical role of cue exposure durations on detecting attentional bias in depression, some studies have examined this factor using different exposure durations but obtained inconsistent conclusions. For example, Zhong et al. (2011) found that MDD patients exhibited a robust negative bias (increased attention to negative and decreased attention to positive stimuli) when the presentation time of cues was 500 ms, and there was also a trend of this bias when the presentation time was decreased to 100 ms. However, Trapp et al. (2018) reported that attentional bias toward painful facial expressions was only observed in depression at the presentation time of 100 ms rather than 500 ms, and the bias indexes at 100-ms condition were correlated with the depressive mood. Furthermore, some studies even observed the negative attentional bias using subliminally exposed cues (17 ms; Miskowiak et al., 2015). The null or non-significant finding at a short cue exposure time (e.g., 100 ms) in the aforementioned studies might be due to the affective materials used for the cues, that is, Donaldson et al. (2007) and Neshat-Doost et al. (2000) used negative words, and Zhong et al. (2011) used affective pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System (Lang et al., 2005). In our opinion, affective facial expressions are more suitable for detecting the negative attentional bias in the case of short cue exposure durations (Stevens et al., 2011; Syrjänen et al., 2019), because facial expressions contain inherent, salient, and evolutionarily adaptive information that should be rapidly perceived and responded by humans (Ekman, 1993).

Another possibility for the null finding at a short exposure time might be the relative insensitivity of the behavioral measure, e.g., the reaction time (RT). Furthermore, behavioral indexes alone could not reveal how the attention modulation of emotions works at the early perceptual stage and at the later top-down integration/regulation stage. The event-related potentials (ERPs) reflect brain responses directly with a high temporal resolution and is considered an ideal method for tracking the dynamics of neural activities in distinct cognitive stages. Using the ERP technique in the dot-probe task, our group has proposed the two-stage model of attentional modulation by emotions (Liu et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017): on the early stage (100–200 ms), the bottom-up pathway (involving the amygdala back-projects to sensory cortices) functions as a response scaling of sensory processing (reflected by the P1 component), which may magnify the sensory perception of emotional, compared with neural, information. On the later stage (250 ms and thereafter), the top-down integration pathway (involving the frontoparietal route to the amygdala) plays a critical role in modulating behaviors and regulating emotions (reflected by the P3 component). In light of these findings (Liu et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017), this study focused on the P1 and P3 components in depressed patients. In general, the occipital P1 (peaking at 100–150 ms post stimuli) is an attention-related component (Hillyard et al., 1998) and has been proved to be sensitive to early emotional modulation in visual perception (Schupp et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2014). Meanwhile, the parietal P3 (peaking at 250–600 ms) is a task-relevant component (Polich, 2007) and often considered as an index of conscious and elaborate evaluation of emotional stimuli (Schupp et al., 2004).

The dot-probe studies focusing on healthy people have revealed that automatic attention attraction by emotionally stimuli reliably enhances the P1, while participants may also explicitly pay more attention to emotionally significant stimuli, reflected by an increased P3 (Pollak and Tolley-Schell, 2003; Pourtois et al., 2004; Brosch et al., 2008). As far as we know, three studies have combined the dot-probe task with ERPs to investigate the attentional bias in MDD patients. Among them, two studies (Hu et al., 2017; Li et al., 2018) selected a 500-ms cue exposure time and mainly examined the attention disengaging difficulty of patients (the P3), which found that the dot-evoked P3 amplitude was enhanced following sad faces in MDD participants. The other study (Zhong et al., 2011) used affective pictures and examined the P1 results in both 100- and 500-ms cue exposure conditions. The authors found that while the healthy group exhibited enhanced P1 amplitudes following positive pictures, this pattern did not appear in the MDD group; furthermore, the observed group difference was significant in the 500-ms condition and showed a trend in the 100-ms condition (Zhong et al., 2011).

As mentioned above, attentional biases consisting of increased attention to negative and reduced attention to positive stimuli are postulated in depression but have rarely been tested for early attentional processing (Trapp et al., 2018). In order to examine the negative bias during initial attentional orientation in MDD patients, this study set the cue exposure time as 100 ms and used mood-congruent (sad) as well as mood-incongruent (happy) facial expressions as the cue stimuli. We hypothesized that MDD patients would show depression-relevant negative bias during the initial attentional allocation stage besides during the well-known attention disengaging stage. In particular, patients would display enhanced responses to sad faces and may also show reduced processing of happy faces, which might be evidenced by an enhanced P1/P3 amplitude following sad faces and/or a reduced P1/P3 amplitude following happy faces when compared with healthy controls.

Materials and Methods

Participants

During the experimental design, we conducted a priori power analysis using GPower 3.1.7 (F tests, ANOVA: repeated measures, within-between interaction) based on the effect size (meanηp20.12) reported in previous related studies (Zhong et al., 2011; Li et al., 2018). According to the result of this power analysis, 12 participants in total would ensure 80% statistical power. However, six participants per group is such a small sample size in present-day neuroscience studies. Thus, we finally decided to include 25 participants per group, which ensured a statistical power near 100%. As a result, 25 inpatients with MDD in Shenzhen Kangning Hospital and 25 healthy controls were recruited from clinics and through advertisements in the community. There was no significant difference between the two groups with respect to age, handedness, and education (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data of patient and control groups.

Patients were diagnosed with a current major depressive episode according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). The diagnosis was based on structured clinical interview for DSM (SCID; First et al., 1996a) and chart review. In addition, all MDD participants were with a score of ≥14 on the Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) in the time of experiment1. Exclusion criteria were neurological disorders and any comorbid Axis I disorders. Particularly, in view of the fact that anxiety and depressive symptoms are highly comorbid (Mineka et al., 1998; Kessler et al., 2005), and that many studies found a correlation between anxiety and abnormality of attention modulation of emotion (e.g., Mueller et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2015; Ho et al., 2017), we only recruited MDD patients without a diagnosis of anxiety disorder in this study. Furthermore, MDD patients with psychotic features, bipolar disorder, or Axis II disorders were excluded. The interview and clinical symptom rating were based on consensus of two senior psychiatrists who were trained with a relatively high reliability (κ = 0.87). At the time of experiment, the 25 patients were either untreated with any antidepressant medication or had undergone a washout period of at least 2 weeks.

Healthy control participants were screened for current Axis I and II disorders using the SCID-I/NP (First et al., 2002) and SCID-II (First et al., 1996b). They were additionally required to have a BDI-II score of ≤10. Exclusion criteria for both MDD and control participants were (1) seizure disorder, (2) history of head injury with possible neurological sequelae, and (3) substance abuse or dependence in the past 6 months. Participants were told about the content of the experiment. Written informed consent was obtained prior to the experiment. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Shenzhen Kangning Hospital (code number: 2017-00264).

Stimuli

Facial pictures were selected from the native Chinese Facial Affective Picture System (Gong et al., 2011), with an equal number of facial pictures of males and females. A total of 80 faces (20 happy, 20 sad, and 40 neutral faces) were used. Each picture had been assessed for its valence and arousal on a 9-point scale with a large sample of Chinese participants in a previous survey (Gong et al., 2011). The one-way ANOVA performed on the average scores of the 80 faces showed that the three categories of faces have significantly different emotional valence [F(2,77) = 143, p < 0.001, ηp2 0.787; happy = 5.92 ± 0.13; sad = 2.78 ± 0.13; neutral = 4.22 ± 0.09; pairwise comparisons: ps < 0.001] as well as arousal scores [F(2,77) = 30.2, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.439; happy = 5.13 ± 0.22; sad = 5.83 ± 0.22; neutral = 3.82 ± 0.16; pairwise comparisons: emotional vs. neutral ps < 0.001, happy vs. sad p = 0.087].

All faces were gray-scale photographs. They were presented with the same contrast and brightness on the black background (3.0° × 3.5° visual angle). The target was a white dot (1.0° × 1.0° visual angle).

Procedure

The experiment consisted of two blocks (happy and sad blocks), each containing 100 trials. The order of the blocks was counterbalanced across subjects.

The design of the dot-probe task was very similar to that used in previous studies (Pourtois et al., 2004; Brosch et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2015). As shown in Figure 1, each trial started with a 300-to-600-ms fixation, followed by a 100-ms cue that consisted of two faces. In the happy block, the cue included a happy and a neutral face; in the sad blocks, the cue included a sad face and a neutral face. Each face was presented five times in a random order in corresponding blocks. The location of the neutral face in each trial was equal for the left or right side. After the cue and a short interval (100–300 ms), the target was presented with the duration of 150 ms. In valid trials, the target appeared at the location previously occupied by the emotional face; in invalid trials, the target appeared at the location previously occupied by the neutral face. Valid and invalid trials were presented in random order with equal probability (50 vs. 50%). Subjects were required to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible regarding the location of the dot target on the computer keyboard (the “F” key for the left location and the “J” key for the right location). The response screen would not disappear until a button press or until 2,000 ms elapsed. Responses with latencies of less than 2,000 ms were considered valid.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of one experimental trial in this study.

Electroencephalography Recording and Analysis

Brain electrical activity was recorded referentially against left mastoid, by a 32-channel amplifier with a sampling frequency of 250 Hz (NeuroScan, Inc., Herndon, VA, United States). To make the results comparable with those of our previous study (Liu et al., 2015), the data were offline re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids. Besides electrooculogram electrodes, 30-channel electroencephalography (EEG) data were collected with electrode impedances kept below 5 kΩ. Ocular artifacts were removed from EEGs by using a regression procedure implemented in NeuroScan software (Scan 4.3).

The data analysis and result display in this study were performed by using Matlab R2011a (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States). The recorded EEG data were filtered with a 0.1-to-20-Hz finite impulse response filter with a zero phase distortion. Filtered data were segmented beginning 100 ms prior to the onset of targets (dots) and lasting for 1,000 ms. All epochs were baseline-corrected with respect to the mean voltage over the 100 ms preceding the onset of targets, followed by averaging in association with experimental conditions.

We analyzed the amplitudes of occipital P1 and parietal P3 components across different sets of electrodes in accordance previous literature (Zhang et al., 2013, 2017; Luck, 2014; Liu et al., 2015; Gu et al., 2018). The mean amplitude of P1 was calculated at the electrode sites of O1 and O2 (time window = 120–150 ms). The mean amplitude of P3 was calculated at Pz, P3, P4, CP1, and CP2 (time window = 250–450 ms).

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed by using SPSS Statistics 20.0 (IBM, Somers, NY, United States). Descriptive data were presented as mean ± standard error. The significance level was set at 0.05. Three-way repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed on accuracy rate, RT, the P1 amplitude, and the P3 amplitude, with emotion of the faces (happy vs. sad) and cue validity (valid vs. invalid) as within-subject factors, and group (MDD patients vs. controls). Significant interactions were analyzed by using a simple-effects model. For the sake of brevity, the effects that did not reach significance have been omitted. Two-tailed Pearson’s r correlation was performed between depression score (measured by BDI-II) and behavioral/ERP data. Correction for multiple comparisons was based on Holm’s stepwise method.

Results

Behavioral Measures

Accuracy Rate

The accuracy rate (ACC) in all conditions was above 98%. The only significant effect was the main effect of cue validity [F(1,48) = 8.59, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.152]: the accuracy was higher in the validly (99.2 ± 0.2%) relative to the invalidly cued condition (98.7 ± 0.3%).

Reaction Time

The main effect of cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 20.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.301; valid vs. invalid = 218.2 ± 7.8 vs. 223.9 ± 8.0 ms]. In addition, the interaction effect of emotion by cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 5.11, p = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.096].

The most important finding is the significant interaction of emotion × cue validity × group [F(1,48) = 4.59, p = 0.037, ηp2 = 0.087; Figure 2A]. To break down the three-way interaction, we tested the two-way interaction of cue validity × group in the happy and sad blocks separately. When the cue contained sad faces, not only the main effect of cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 19.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.285; valid vs. invalid = 217.2 ± 8.3 vs. 226.9 ± 9.1 ms], but also the cue validity × group interaction was marginally significant [F(1,48) = 3.73, p = 0.059, ηp2 = 0.072]. Simple-effects analysis revealed that while patients responded faster for the validly than invalidly cued target [F(1,48) = 19.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.293; valid vs. invalid = 211.9 ± 11.8 vs. 225.9 ± 12.9 ms], this effect was not found in the control group [F(1,48) = 2.98, p = 0.091, ηp2 = 0.058; valid vs. invalid = 222.5 ± 11.8 vs. 228.0 ± 12.9 ms]. However, when the cue contained happy faces, neither the main effect of cue validity (F < 1) nor the two-way interaction was significant [F(1,48) = 2.45, p = 0.124, ηp2 = 0.049].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The reaction time, P1 amplitude, and P3 amplitude. (A) The reaction time. (B) The P1 amplitude. (C) The P3 amplitude. Bars represent standard error of the mean.

Event-Related Potential Measures

P1 Amplitudes

The main effect of emotion was significant [F(1,48) = 16.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.260; happy vs. sad = 2.04 ± 0.32 vs. 3.20 ± 0.39 μV]. Also, the main effect of cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 13.9, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.224; valid vs. invalid = 3.09 ± 0.35 vs. 2.16 ± 0.35 μV].

Importantly, the interaction of emotion × cue validity × group was significant [F(1,48) = 5.27, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.099; Figures 2B, 3]. To break down the three-way interaction, we tested the two-way interaction of cue validity × group in the happy and sad blocks separately. When the cue contained happy faces, the cue validity × group interaction was marginally significant [F(1,48) = 3.82, p = 0.057, ηp2 = 0.074]. Simple-effects analysis revealed that while controls showed larger P1 amplitudes for valid than invalid happy cues [F(1,48) = 6.55, p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.120; valid vs. invalid = 2.92 ± 0.52 vs. 1.58 ± 0.53 μV], this effect was not significant in the patient group [F(1,48) < 1; valid vs. invalid = 1.78 ± 0.52 vs. 1.89 ± 0.53 μV]. However, when the cue contained sad faces, only the main effect of cue validity was significant for both groups [F(1,48) = 15.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.246; valid vs. invalid = 3.82 ± 0.41 vs. 2.58 ± 0.43 μV].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The grand-mean event-related potential (ERP) waveforms of P1 component in the two groups (data averaged at the occipital electrode site of O1 and O2).

P3 Amplitudes

The interaction effect of cue validity by group was significant [F(1,48) = 7.35, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.133]. Also, the interaction effect of emotion × cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 9.53, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.166].

Importantly, the interaction effect of emotion × cue validity × group was significant [F(1,48) = 9.25, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.162; Figures 2C, 4]. To break down the three-way interaction, we tested the two-way interaction of cue validity × group in the happy and sad blocks separately. When the cue contained sad faces, the cue validity × group interaction was significant [F(1,48) = 24.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.341]. Simple-effects analysis revealed that while patients showed larger P3 amplitudes for valid than invalid sad cues [F(1,48) = 5.73, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.107; valid vs. invalid = 6.91 ± 1.23 vs. 6.01 ± 1.18 μV], the pattern was reversed in the control group; i.e., they had larger P3 amplitudes for invalid than valid sad cues [F(1,48) = 21.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.311; valid vs. invalid = 6.17 ± 1.23 vs. 7.93 ± 1.18 μV]. However, when the cue contained happy faces, only the main effect of cue validity was significant [F(1,48) = 6.23, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.115; valid vs. invalid = 8.06 ± 0.87 vs. 7.11 ± 0.90 μV].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The grand-mean event-related potential (ERP) waveforms of P3 component in the two groups (data averaged at the parietal electrode site of Pz, P3, P4, CP1, and CP2).

Correlation

According to the results reported above, three correlations were performed between the depression score (measured by BDI-II) and the three behavioral/ERP indexes (i.e., the RT and the P3 amplitude in the sad condition, and the P1 amplitude in the happy condition). To directly examine the attention modulation of emotions, we calculated the difference measurement between validly and invalidly cued conditions (attentional bias score; see also Lubman et al., 2000; Townshend and Duka, 2001; Liu et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017).

As a result, the RT bias (invalid–valid) in the sad cue condition was correlated with the BDI score (r = 0.366, p = 0.009, corrected p = 0.018; Figure 5A). The P1 amplitude bias (invalid–valid) in the happy cue condition was correlated with the BDI score (r = 0.342, p = 0.015, corrected p = 0.015; Figure 5B). The P3 amplitude bias (valid–invalid) in the sad cue condition was correlated with the BDI score (r = 0.383, p = 0.006, corrected p = 0.018; Figure 5C).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The correlation between the depression score [Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition (BDI-II)] and behavioral/event-related potential (ERP) indexes. (A) The reaction time (RT) bias (invalid–valid) in the sad cue condition is correlated with the BDI score. (B) The P1 amplitude bias (valid–invalid) in the sad cue condition was correlated with the BDI score. (C) The P3 amplitude bias (valid–invalid) in the sad cue condition was correlated with the BDI score.

Discussion

While the negative attentional bias has been well established during attention maintenance and disengaging in MDD patients (for reviews, see Mathews and MacLeod, 2005; Peckham et al., 2010), there is no converging evidence for whether the negative bias exits during initial attentional orientation and what the cognitive mechanism is underlying this early attention process. Using a 100-ms cue exposure time in the dot-probe task, this study found that the MDD participants (but not the control ones) responded faster following valid compared with invalid sad facial cues, providing the second evidence (following Trapp et al., 2018) that depressed patients show a mood-congruent attentional bias during initial attention allocation. Our finding is consistent with the results reported by Rinck and Becker (2005), who used a visual search task and found that depression-related words were more distracting for the depressed than for control participants. In addition, we also found the main effect of cue validity for both behavioral measures; i.e., the validly cued condition was associated with a higher accuracy rate and a shorter RT than was the invalid condition, indicating a successful manipulation of cue validity in this study.

Beyond Trapp et al. (2018), this study contributes to the growing body of research aimed to reveal the underpinning of attentional bias in depression using ERP indexes that are sensitive to distinct attention allocation stages. Our results show that the MDD patients exhibited enhanced P1 amplitudes following validly than invalidly sad cues, whereas the healthy controls showed enhanced P1 amplitudes for both validly (compared with invalidly) happy and sad cued targets. The P1 has been associated with rapid and unconscious bottom-up attention allocation (Luck and Kappenman, 2012). Previous ERP studies have demonstrated that automatic attention attraction by emotionally significant stimuli could reliably enhance the P1 amplitudes in the dot-probe task, representing a facilitated perception in response to the target presented at the already attended location (Hopfinger and Mangun, 1998; Pourtois et al., 2004; Brosch et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2015). The P1 finding of this study suggests that during an early stage of bottom-up response scaling of sensory processing, MDD patients show decreased attention for positive stimuli than do healthy people. This result is in line with the evenhanded theory (Gotlib and McCabe, 1992; McCabe and Gotlib, 1995), which argued that while healthy people are usually more desirable to attend to positive stimuli (compared with neutral ones) and display optimistic bias that is self-protective and good for mental health, depressed people, by contrast, lack this protective, positive bias, so they show no difference in attentional allocation between positive and neutral information (Korn et al., 2014; Winer and Salem, 2016). The current P1 results not only are consistent with previous behavioral findings in depression (Gotlib et al., 1988; Mogg et al., 1991; Koster et al., 2005; Kellough et al., 2008; Atchley et al., 2012; for reviews see Peckham et al., 2010; Winer and Salem, 2016) but also restrict the attentional impairment within the P1 time window, i.e., on the stage of automatic bottom-up attention allocation.

Unlike the P1, the P3 component has been proven to reflect top-down modulation and voluntary allocation of attention (Fichtenholtz et al., 2007; Polich, 2007; Leutgeb et al., 2015), and a larger P3 in the dot-probe task indicates explicit attention being paid to emotionally significant stimuli (Pollak and Tolley-Schell, 2003). The main finding at the P3 of this study is that the MDD patients exhibited enhanced P3 amplitudes following validly than invalidly sad cues, whereas the control participants showed an opposite results, i.e., displayed enhanced P3 amplitudes following invalidly than validly sad cues. According to the two-stage model of attentional modulation by negative emotions (Liu et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017), the attention modulation of emotions at the later stage is voluntarily controlled; thus, attention could be flexibly tuned to fit cognitive goals with the least effort. Therefore, the enhanced P3 for invalid sad cues in the control group indicated that more cognitive resources were allocated toward the task-relevant but previously less attended location, in order to ensure the effective achievement of task goals. However, the MDD patients paid more attention to valid than invalid sad cues on both stages of automatic and voluntary attention allocation, indicating a negative, mood-congruent attention bias in depression (Peckham et al., 2010; Yiend, 2010; Armstrong and Olatunji, 2012; Koster and Hoorelbeke, 2015; Winer and Salem, 2016). Similar with this finding, other studies using relevant tasks also reported enhanced attention and P3 amplitudes for negative stimuli in depressed as compared with healthy individuals (Atchley et al., 2003; Ilardi et al., 2007). In addition, this study found that on the stage of top-down voluntary modulation of attention, both MDD patients and healthy controls showed enhanced P3 amplitudes following validly than invalidly happy cues, though due to distinct reasons. While the patients reallocated more attention toward the previously less attended happy-cued target in order to perform well in the task, healthy participants continued to assign more attention resources to the location of happy compared with neutral cues due to their protective, positive bias (Gotlib and McCabe, 1992; Korn et al., 2014; Winer and Salem, 2016).

Taken together, the findings of behavioral and ERP measures indicate that MDD patients show mood-congruent bias in early attention allocation processes, reflected by the sad-prone indexes of RT and P3 amplitudes, and the happy-avoidance index of P1 amplitudes. Interestingly, the attentional bias scores of these three indexes were correlated well with the depression level measured by the BDI-II (Figure 5). These findings have valuable clinical implications for both impairment titration and timely evaluation of treatment effects. For instance, many studies used the procedure attention bias modification to alleviate the negative bias in depression (for reviews, see Hallion and Ruscio, 2011; Mogoaşe et al., 2014), which found that reducing the attentional bias score based on RT could contribute to the recovery process for MDD patients (Zhou et al., 2015) and causally reduce the chance of depressive recurrence (Browning et al., 2012). This study further provides electrophysiological indexes in complementary with previously employed behavioral ones. The advantage is that the attentional bias scores based on the amplitudes of the P1 and the P3 could separately and specifically tell whether and to what extent the deficits in automatic (the P1) and voluntary (the P3) attentional allocation are in patients, so as to provide a more detailed instruction for individualized treatment.

Besides clinical implications, the current findings also facilitate the understanding of cognitive mechanism of attentional modulation by emotions. Previously, we used threat-related facial expressions as cues (anger, fear, and disgust) and proposed the two-stage model of attentional modulation by emotions (Liu et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017). In this study, the examination of another two basic emotions, i.e., sadness and happiness, added important knowledge to this cognitive model. On the early stage of bottom-up response scaling of sensory processing (reflected by the P1), the brain magnifies (anger, fear, sadness, and happiness) or minifies (disgust) the sensory perception of emotions by automatically allocating increased or decreased attention resources toward the location of emotional stimuli, in accordance with the evolutionary purposes of various emotions. On the later stage of top-down attention control (reflected by the P3), attention is voluntarily tuned to fit task goals with least effort (negative emotions) or maintained upon the emotional stimuli in order to achieve a status of mental well-being (positive emotions).

Finally, one limitation of this study should be noticed. Although a large amount of studies have shown that a 1,000-ms cue could reliably evoke negative attentional bias in depression, it is preferred to directly compare the attentional bias effects in 100- and 1,000-ms conditions within one study. This could be one of the further directions to facilitate a comprehensive understanding of the negative attentional bias in depression.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study examined the depression-relevant negative bias during initial attentional allocation in MDD patients. The ERP measures revealed that (1) during an early stage of automatic attention allocation (reflected by the P1 amplitude), MDD patients showed decreased attention for positive stimuli than did healthy people; and (2) they allocated enhanced attention toward mood-congruent sad stimuli than did controls on the later stage of top-down voluntary control of attention (reflected by the P3 amplitude). The attentional bias scores of the P1 and the P3 amplitudes were correlated with the depression levels, providing electrophysiological indexes in distinct (early automatic vs. later voluntary) attentional allocation stages to guide the clinical treatment of depression. Furthermore, the findings in the healthy control group enriched the two-stage model of attentional modulation by emotions and deepened our understanding of cognitive mechanism underlying the emotional influence on attention.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Shenzhen Kangning Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DZ and ZW designed the study. ZW, WY, and FZ collected the experimental data. DZ analyzed the data. XA and LM wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31970980 and 31920103009), Shenzhen Basic Research Project (JCYJ20180305124305294), Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science (2019SHIBS0003), and Guangdong Key Basic Research Grant (2018B0 30332001).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Note that while BDI-II < 14 indicates minimal depression, BDI-II ≥ 14 indicates mild, moderate, or severe depression.

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Armstrong, T., and Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnone, D., McKie, S., Elliott, R., Thomas, E. J., Downey, D., Juhasz, G., et al. (2012). Increased amygdala responses to sad but not fearful faces in major depression: relation to mood state and pharmacological treatment. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 841–850. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11121774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atchley, R. A., Ilardi, S. S., and Enloe, A. (2003). Hemispheric asymmetry in the lexical processing of emotion: the effect of current and past depression. Brain Lang. 84, 105–119. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00523-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atchley, R. A., Ilardi, S. S., Young, K. M., Stroupe, N. N., O’Hare, A. J., Bistricky, S. L., et al. (2012). Depression reduces perceptual sensitivity for positive words and pictures. Cogn. Emot. 26, 1359–1370. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.660134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., and Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 911–927. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00053-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., and Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychol. Sci. 19, 362–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, H. M., Eley, T. C., Broeren, S., Macleod, C., Rinck, M., Hadwin, J. A., et al. (2014). Psychometric properties of reaction time based experimental paradigms measuring anxiety-related information-processing biases in children. J. Anxiety Disord. 28, 97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browning, M., Holmes, E. A., Charles, M., Cowen, P. J., and Harmer, C. J. (2012). Using attentional bias modification as a cognitive vaccine against depression. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 572–579. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, R. M., and Langton, S. R. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1321–1329. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Raedt, R., and Koster, E. H. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 50–70. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A., and Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 467–477. doi: 10.1038/nrn3027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donaldson, C., Lam, D., and Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2664–2678. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. Am. Psychol. 48, 384–392.

Google Scholar

Ferrari, G. R., Becker, E. S., Smit, F., Rinck, M., and Spijker, J. (2016). Investigating the (cost-) effectiveness of attention bias modification (ABM) for outpatients with major depressive disorder (MDD): a randomized controlled trial protocol. BMC Psychiatry 16:370. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1085-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fichtenholtz, H. M., Hopfinger, J. B., Graham, R., Detwiler, J. M., and LaBar, K. S. (2007). Happy and fearful emotion in cues and targets modulate event-related potential indices of gaze-directed attentional orienting. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 323–333. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., Williams, J., and Benjamin, L. (1996a). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Patient Edition. SCID - I/P. New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Google Scholar

First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., Williams, J., and Benjamin, L. (1996b). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Google Scholar

First, M., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. L., and Williams, J. (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Google Scholar

Fritzsche, A., Dahme, B., Gotlib, I. H., Joormann, J., Magnussen, H., Watz, H., et al. (2010). Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients with asthma. Psychol. Med. 40, 815–826. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Blanco, A., Salmerón, L., Perea, M., and Livianos, L. (2014). Attentional biases toward emotional images in the different episodes of bipolar disorder: an eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Res. 215, 628–633. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, X., Huang, Y., Wang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2011). Revision of the Chinese facial affective picture system. Chin. Ment. Health J. 25, 40–46. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2011.01.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., and Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Arnow, B. A., and Johnson, S. L. (2004a). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 386–398. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., and Joormann, J. (2004b). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113, 121–135. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., and McCabe, S. B. (1992). An information-processing approach to the study of cognitive functioning in depression. Prog. Exp. Personal. Psychopathol. Res. 15, 131–161.

Google Scholar

Gotlib, I. H., McLachlan, A. L., and Katz, A. N. (1988). Biases in visual attention in depressed and nondepressed individuals. Cogn. Emot. 2, 185–200. doi: 10.1080/02699938808410923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, R., Zhang, D., Luo, Y., Wang, H., and Broster, L. S. (2018). Predicting risk decisions in a modified balloon analogue risk task: conventional and single-trial ERP analyses. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 99–116. doi: 10.3758/s13415-017-0555-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Günther, V., Dannlowski, U., Kersting, A., and Suslow, T. (2015). Associations between childhood maltreatment and emotion processing biases in major depression: results from a dot-probe task. BMC Psychiatry 15:123. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0501-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallion, L. S., and Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol. Bull. 137:940. doi: 10.1037/a0024355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., Abela, J. R., and Flory, K. (2010). Selective attention to affective stimuli and clinical depression among youths: role of anxiety and specificity of emotion. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 491–501. doi: 10.1037/a0019609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., and Luck, S. J. (1998). Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B Biol. Sci. 353, 1257–1270. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, S., Yeung, D., and Mak, C. (2017). The interaction effect of attentional bias and attentional control on dispositional anxiety among adolescents. Br. J. Psychol. 108, 564–582. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopfinger, J. B., and Mangun, G. R. (1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychol. Sci. 9, 441–447. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, B., Rao, J., Li, X., Cao, T., Li, J., Majoe, D., et al. (2017). Emotion regulating attentional control abnormalities in major depressive disorder: an event-related potential study. Sci. Rep. 7:13530. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13626-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilardi, S. S., Atchley, R. A., Enloe, A., Kwasny, K., and Garratt, G. (2007). Disentangling attentional biases and attentional deficits in depression: an event-related potential P300 analysis. Cognit. Ther. Res. 31, 175–187. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9113-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., and Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 887–900. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joormann, J., and Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 80–85. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kappenman, E. S., Farrens, J. L., Luck, S. J., and Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety. Front. Psychol. 5:1368. doi: 10.3389/2014.01368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., and Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: an eye tracking study. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1238–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korn, C. W., Sharot, T., Walter, H., Heekeren, H. R., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). Depression is related to an absence of optimistically biased belief updating about future life events. Psychol. Med. 44, 579–592. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., and Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion 5, 446–455. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, E. H., and Hoorelbeke, K. (2015). Cognitive bias modification for depression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 4, 119–123. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. Technical report A-6. Gainesvile, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Le, M. M., Cowen, P. J., and Harmer, C. J. (2007). Emotional bias and waking salivary cortisol in relatives of patients with major depression. Psychol. Med. 37, 403–410. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leutgeb, V., Sarlo, M., Schöngassner, F., and Schienle, A. (2015). Out of sight, but still in mind: electrocortical correlates of attentional capture in spider phobia as revealed by a ‘dot probe’ paradigm. Brain Cogn. 93, 26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyman, L., De, R. R., Schacht, R., and Koster, E. H. (2007). Attentional biases for angry faces in unipolar depression. Psychol. Med. 37, 393–402. doi: 10.1017/S003329170600910X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Li, J., Hu, B., Zhu, J., Zhang, X., Wei, L., et al. (2018). Attentional bias in MDD: ERP components analysis and classification using a dot-probe task. Comp. Methods Programs 164, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2018.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, T., Vaisvaser, S., Fruchter, E., Admon, R., Wald, I., Pine, D. S., et al. (2015). A neurobehavioral account for individual differences in resilience to chronic military stress. Psychol. Med. 45, 1011–1023. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Zhang, D., and Luo, Y. (2015). How disgust facilitates avoidance: an ERP study on attention modulation by threats. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 598–604. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubman, D. I., Peters, L. A., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., and Deakin, J. F. (2000). Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychol. Med. 30, 169–175. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799001269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J. (2014). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique, 2nd Edn. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Kappenman, E. S. (2012). ERP Components and Selective Attention. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, 295–327. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., and Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, A., and MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1, 167–195. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., and Williamson, D. A. (1996). Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 695–705. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00046-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, S. B., and Gotlib, I. H. (1995). Selective attention and clinical depression: performance on a deployment-of-attention task. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104, 241–245. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.104.1.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mineka, S., Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 377–412. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miskowiak, K. W., Glerup, L., Vestbo, C., Harmer, C. J., Reinecke, A., Macoveanu, J., et al. (2015). Different neural and cognitive response to emotional faces in healthy monozygotic twins at risk of depression. Psychol. Med. 45, 1447–1458. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., and Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 34, 17–36. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., Mathews, A., May, J., Grove, M., Eysenck, M., and Weinman, J. (1991). Assessment of cognitive bias in anxiety and depression using a colour perception task. Cogn. Emot. 5, 221–238. doi: 10.1080/02699939108411036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogoaşe, C., David, D., and Koster, E. H. (2014). Clinical efficacy of attentional bias modification procedures: an updated meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 1133–1157. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, E. M., Hofmann, S. G., Santesso, D. L., Meuret, A. E., Bitran, S., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence of attentional biases in social anxiety disorder. Psychol. Med. 39, 1141–1152. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neshat-Doost, H. T., Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Yule, W., and Dalgleish, T. (2000). Lack of attentional bias for emotional information in clinically depressed children and adolescents on the dot probe task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 363–368. doi: 10.1017/s0021963099005429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peckham, A. D., McHugh, R. K., and Otto, M. W. (2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress. Anxiety 27, 1135–1142. doi: 10.1002/da.20755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., and Ungerleider, L. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 11458–11463. doi: 10.1073/pnas.172403899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollak, S. D., and Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 323–335. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., and Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 109, 160–174. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., and Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cereb. Cortex 14, 619–633. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pourtois, G., and Vuilleumier, P. (2006). Dynamics of emotional effects on spatial attention in the human visual cortex. Prog. Brain Res. 156, 67–91. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56004-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinck, M., and Becker, E. S. (2005). A comparison of attentional biases and memory biases in women with social phobia and major depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 62–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., and Hamm, A. O. (2004). The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: an ERP analysis. Psychophysiology 41, 441–449. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00174.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., and Hamm, A. O. (2007). Selective visual attention to emotion. J. Neurosci. 27, 1082–1089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3223-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K., and De Torres, I. B. C. (2014). A world of depression. Nature 515:181. doi: 10.1038/515180a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Stevens, S., Rist, F., and Gerlach, A. L. (2011). Eye movement assessment in individuals with social phobia: differential usefulness for varying presentation times? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 42, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syrjänen, E., Fischer, H., and Olofsson, J. K. (2019). Background odors affect behavior in a dot-probe task with emotionally expressive faces. Physiol. Behav. 10112540. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townshend, J., and Duka, T. (2001). Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology 157, 67–74. doi: 10.1007/s002130100764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trapp, W., Kalzendorf, C., Baum, C., and Hajak, G. (2018). Lautenbacher S. Attentional biases in patients suffering from unipolar depression: results of a dot probe task investigation. Psychiatry Res. 261, 325–331. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30, 829–841. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winer, E. S., and Salem, T. (2016). Reward devaluation: dot-probe meta-analytic evidence of avoidance of positive information in depressed persons. Psychol. Bull. 142, 18–78. doi: 10.1037/bul0000022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: a review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cogn. Emot. 24, 3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Gu, R., Wu, T., Broster, L. S., Luo, Y., Jiang, Y., et al. (2013). An electrophysiological index of changes in risk decision-making strategies. Neuropsychologia 51, 1397–1407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., He, W., Wang, T., Luo, W., Zhu, X., Gu, R., et al. (2014). Three stages of emotional word processing: an ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1897–1903. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Liu, Y., Wang, L., Ai, H., and Luo, Y. (2017). Mechanisms for attentional modulation by threatening emotions of fear, anger, and disgust. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 198–210. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0473-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Q., Li, H., Hu, B., Li, Y., Gillebert, C., Mantini, D., et al. (2017). Neural correlates of drug-related attentional bias in heroin dependence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:646. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, M., Zhu, X., Yi, J., Yao, S., and Ruth, A. A. (2011). Do the early attentional components of ERPs reflect attentional bias in depression? It depends on the stimulus presentation time. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1371–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Cao, S., Li, H., and Li, Y. (2015). Treatment with escitalopram improves the attentional bias toward negative facial expressions in patients with major depressive disorders. J. Clin. Neurosci. 22, 1609–1613. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2015.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: major depressive disorder, attentional bias, mood congruent, dot-probe task 3, initial attentional allocation

Citation: Ao X, Mo L, Wei Z, Yu W, Zhou F and Zhang D (2020) Negative Bias During Early Attentional Engagement in Major Depressive Disorder as Examined Using a Two-Stage Model: High Sensitivity to Sad but Bluntness to Happy Cues. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:593010. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.593010

Received: 09 August 2020; Accepted: 21 October 2020;
Published: 17 November 2020.

Edited by:

Carmen Moret-Tatay, Catholic University of Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Spain

Reviewed by:

Junming Shao, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Andrea Lami, Catholic University of Valencia San Vicente M rtir, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Ao, Mo, Wei, Yu, Zhou and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dandan Zhang, zhangdd05@gmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Download