Skip to main content
Log in

Human Males Appear More Prepared Than Females to Resolve Conflicts with Same-Sex Peers

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of the study was to investigate sex differences in proximate mechanisms that precede the termination of conflicts. In Study 1, we asked women and men to report their intensity of anger in response to hypothetical, common transgressions involving a same-sex roommate. Direct verbal and physical aggression elicited the highest-intensity anger for both sexes, although overall women reported more intense anger than men to all transgressions. In Study 2, we examined sex differences in subjective and physiological reactions to a conflict using a role-playing scenario. Following recall of a conflict involving direct aggression and role-playing a reaction to it, compared with men, women reported their anger would dissipate less quickly and they would take longer to reconcile. Women also exhibited increased heart rate, but little change in cortisol, whereas men exhibited little change in heart rate but increased cortisol production. We interpret the results as indicating that women are less prepared than men to resolve a conflict with a same-sex peer.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Archer, J. (2000). Sex Differences in Aggression Between Heterosexual Partners: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 651–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (2004). Sex Differences in Aggression in Real-World Settings: A Meta-Analytic Review. Review of General Psychology, 8(4), 291–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (2006). Cross-Cultural Differences in Physical Aggression Between Partners: A Social-Role Analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(2), 133–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction: Reconsidering the differences. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. F., & Alavi, K. (2004). Sex Differences in children’s Investment in Same-sex Peers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(4), 258–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. F., & Christakos, A. (2003). The Greater Fragility of females’ Versus males’ Closest Same-sex Friendships. Child Development, 74(4), 1123–1129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J., Saelen, C., Markovits, H., & McCabe, S. (2008). Males Derive Greater Benefits Than Females do from Same-sex Peers Relative to Parents. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 13–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. F., Markovits, H., Fitzgerald, C., Geoffroy, D., Flemming, J., Kahlenberg, S. M., et al. (2009). Males’ Greater Tolerance of Same-sex Peers. Psychological Science, 20(2), 184–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J., Markovits, H., Hultgren, B., Nyugen, T., Bullock, G., & Wrangham, R. W. (2013). Females More Than Males Engage in Social Exclusion. PloS One, 8(2), e55851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackhart, G. C., Nelson, B. C., Knowles, M. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Rejection Elicits Emotional Reactions but Neither Causes Immediate Distress nor Lowers Self-Esteem: A Meta-Analytic Review of 192 Studies on Social Exclusion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(4), 269–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (2009). Did Warfare Among Ancestral Hunter-Gatherers Affect the Evolution of Human Social Behaviors? Science, 324(5932), 1293–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L. (2008). Reconciliation, Dominance and Cortisol Levels in Children and Adolescents (7–15 Year-old Boys). Behaviour, 145(11), 1557–1576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L., & Kozinstev, A. (1999). Aggression, Friendship, and Reconciliation in Russian Primary Schoolchildren. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 125–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L., Boyko, E. Y., Selverova, N. B., & Ermakova, I. V. (2005). The Hormonal Basis of Reconciliation in Humans. Journal of Physiological Anthropology and Applied Human Science, 24(4), 333–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L., Timentschik, V., & Burkova, V. N. (2007). Aggression, Conflict Resolution, Popularity, and Attitude to School in Russian Adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 170–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A. (2008). Attachment, Aggression and Affiliation: The Role of Oxytocin in Female Social Behavior. Biological Psychology, 77, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., & Muncer, S. (2008). Intent to Harm or Injure? Gender and the Expression of Anger. Aggressive Behavior, 34(3), 282–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2004). Neural Correlates Ofmental Rehearsal in Dorsal Premotor Cortext. Nature, 431, 993–996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). The Integration of Dominance and Social Bonding in Primates. Quarterly Review of Biology, 61, 459–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & van Roosmalen, A. (1979). Reconciliation and Consolation Among Chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 55–66.

  • Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute Stressors and Cortisol Responses: A Theoretical Integration and Synthesis of Laboratory Research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic Nervous System Activity Distinguishes Among Emotions. Science, 221(4616), 1208–1210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, C., Pascual, J. C., Soler, J., Elices, M., Portella, M. J., & Fernández-Abascal, E. (2012). Physiological Responses Induced by Emotion-Eliciting Films. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 37, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, G. A. (1980). The natural history of preadolescent male friendship groups. In H. C. Foot, A. J. Chapman, & J. R. Smith (Eds.), Friendship and social relations in children (pp. 293–320). Piscataway: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M. V., Ponzi, D., & Muehlenbein, M. P. (2012). Hormonal Mechanisms for Regulation of Aggression in Human Coalitions. Human Nature, 23, 68–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, O. N., Stahl, D., & Aureli, F. (2008). Stress Reduction Through Consolation in Chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academcy of Sciences, 105(25), 8557–8562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furuichi, T. (2011). Contribution of Female Bonobos to the Peaceful Nature of the Society. Evolutionary Anthropology, 20, 131–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are There“his” and“ Hers” Types of Interdependence? The Implications of Gender Differences in Collective Versus Relational Interdependence for Affect, Behavior, and Cognition. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 77(3), 642–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., & Fruth, B. (2000). Use and Function of Genital Contacts Among Female Bonobos. Animal Behaviour, 60(1), 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, J. P., Koslov, K., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). Experiencing Discrimination Increases Risk Taking. Psychological Science, 24, 131–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of Gender, Menstrual Cycle Phase, and Oral Contraceptives on the Activity of the Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51, 154–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kon, I. S., & Losenkov, V. A. (1978). Friendship in Adolescence: Values and Behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 40, 143–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kudielka, B. M., & Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex Differences in HPA Axis Responses to Stress: A Review. Biological Psychology, 69(1), 113–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kudielka, B. M., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). HPA Axis Responses to Laboratory Psychosocial Stress in Healthy Elderly Adults, Younger Adults, and Children: Impact of age and Gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(1), 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langergraber, K., Mitani, J., & Vigilant, L. (2009). Kinship and Social Bonds in Female Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 71(10), 840–851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, J., & Boesch, C. (2009). Sociality of the Dispersing sex: The Nature of Social Bonds in West African Female Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behavior, 77(2), 377–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever, J. (1978). Sex Differences in the Complexity of children’s Play and Games. American Sociological Review, 43(4), 471–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacEvoy, J. P., & Asher, S. R. (2012). When Friends Disappoint: Boys’ and girls’ Responses to Transgressions of Friendship Expectations. Child Development, 83, 104–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markovits, H., Benenson, J., & Dolenszky, E. (2001). Evidence That Children and Adolescents Have Internal Models of Peer Interactions That are Gender Differentiated. Child Development, 72(3), 879–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palagi, E., Paoli, T., & Tarli, S. B. (2004). Reconciliation and Consolation in Captive Bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Primatology, 62(1), 15–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, and self-regulation. New York: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romero, T., Castellanos, M. A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Consolation as Possible Expression of Sympathy Among Chimpanzees. Proccedings of the National Academcy of Sciences, 107, 12110–12115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do Glucocorticoids Influence Stress Responses? Integrating Permissive, Suppressive, Stimulatory, and Preparative Actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 55–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savin-Williams, R. C. (1980). Social interactions of adolescent females in natural groups. In H. C. Foot, A. J. Chapman, & J. R. Smith (Eds.), Friendship and social relations in children (pp. 343–363). New Brunswick: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1991). Adolescence. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, K., & Ross, M. (2010). Why Women Apologize More Than men. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1649–1655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A. N. (2011). The Recalibrational Theory and Violent Anger. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16, 381–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A. N., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the Logic of Human Anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15073–15078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, L. R., Salovey, P., & Epel, E. S. (2002). Sex Differences in Stress Responses: Social Rejection Versus Achievement Stress. Biological Psychiatry, 52(4), 318–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral Responses to Stress in Females: Tend-and-Befriend, not Fight-or-Flight. Psychological Review, 107(3), 411–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiger, L. (1969). Men in groups. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man (pp. 136–179). Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (2006). Conflict Resolution in Chimpanzees and the Valuable-Relationships Hypothesis. International Journal of Primatology, 27(5), 1337–1364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitesell, N. R., & Harter, S. (1996). The Interpersonal Context of Emotion: Anger With Close Friends and Classmates. Child Development, 67(4), 1345–1359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winstead, B., & Griffin, J. (2001). Friendship styles. In J. Worell (Ed.), Encyclopedia of women and gender (pp. 481–492). Boston: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittig, R. M., & Boesch, C. (2003). The Choice of Post-Conflict Interactions in Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behaviour, 140, 1527–1559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W. (1999). Evolution of Coalitionary Killing. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 29(42), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W. (2000). Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In P. M. Kappeler (Ed.), Primate males (pp. 248–258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Emmanuel College, Harvard University, and the University of New Mexico for support of this project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joyce F. Benenson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Benenson, J.F., Kuhn, M.N., Ryan, P.J. et al. Human Males Appear More Prepared Than Females to Resolve Conflicts with Same-Sex Peers. Hum Nat 25, 251–268 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9198-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9198-z

Keywords

Navigation