Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 March 2021
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Songs and Signs: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Cultural Transmission and Inheritance in Human and Nonhuman Animals View all 20 articles

Higher-Order Musical Temporal Structure in Bird Song

\nHans T. Bilger,
Hans T. Bilger1,2*Emily VertosickEmily Vertosick3Andrew VickersAndrew Vickers3Konrad KaczmarekKonrad Kaczmarek4Richard O. PrumRichard O. Prum1
  • 1Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, and Peabody Museum of Natural History, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 2Department of Integrative Biology, University of Texas, Austin, TX, United States
  • 3Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, New York, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Music, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States

Bird songs often display musical acoustic features such as tonal pitch selection, rhythmicity, and melodic contouring. We investigated higher-order musical temporal structure in bird song using an experimental method called “music scrambling” with human subjects. Recorded songs from a phylogenetically diverse group of 20 avian taxa were split into constituent elements (“notes” or “syllables”) and recombined in original and random order. Human subjects were asked to evaluate which version sounded more “musical” on a per-species basis. Species identity and stimulus treatment were concealed from subjects, and stimulus presentation order was randomized within and between taxa. Two recordings of human music were included as a control for attentiveness. Participants varied in their assessments of individual species musicality, but overall they were significantly more likely to rate bird songs with original temporal sequence as more musical than those with randomized temporal sequence. We discuss alternative hypotheses for the origins of avian musicality, including honest signaling, perceptual bias, and arbitrary aesthetic coevolution.

Introduction

Many bird songs show striking behavioral, neural, genetic, and developmental parallels with human language (Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Bolhuis et al., 2010; Bolhuis, 2013; Lipkind et al., 2013; Jarvis, 2019; Hyland Bruno et al., 2020). Like language, bird song can be combinatorial and hierarchical: Elements, or notes, combine to form syllables; syllables are combined into phrases; phrases are combined to form songs; and multiple songs comprise an individual song repertoire (ten Cate and Okanoya, 2012; Bowling and Fitch, 2015). But the linguistic analogy breaks down above this basic scheme. Since bird songs seem to lack symbolic meaning beyond basic functional reference, they cannot have words, semantics, or syntax in the strict linguistic sense (Marler et al., 1992; Berwick et al., 2011; Bowling and Fitch, 2015). To make empirical sense of the structure and content of bird song, we need different conceptual tools.

Music can be defined as “human sound communication outside the scope of spoken language” (Nettl, 2005:25). Although music varies greatly across cultures in its acoustic features, behavioral context, and conceptual framing, it also appears to have remarkable statistically “universal” qualities (Stumpf, 1911; Voss and Clarke, 1975; Nettl, 2005; Savage et al., 2015; Mehr et al., 2019). Analyzing a diverse global ethnographic sample, Mehr et al. (2019) found that human vocal songs: (1) showed more variation within than between societies, (2) were reliably associated with behavioral contexts like love, dance, infant care, religious activity, play, and healing, (3) nearly always displayed tonality, and (4) showed power-law distributions of melodic and rhythmic ratios, where a few frequently used ratios dominate, followed by a “heavy tail” of rarer ones (Adamic, 2011). Bird song has described in musical terms for centuries—in Historia Animalium (Stap, 2005), Aristotle described a nightingale giving “lessons in singing to a young bird”—but until recently, most scientific literature on music and bird song was biologically superficial and overly reliant on Eurocentric conceptions of musical structure (Rothenberg et al., 2014). Convergences between bird song and music were treated as objects of idle charm, and comparative studies often simply examined times when famous Western composers got inspired by birds (e.g., Keister and Baptista, 2005).

However, Darwin (1871), Craig (1943), Armstrong (1963), Hartshorne (1973) and Rothenberg et al. (2014) and others have given serious consideration to the idea that musical aesthetics can provide intellectual insights into avian evolution. Hartshorne (1958, 1973) considered bird song an “evolutionary anticipation of human music,” and used a six-dimensional rating system to quantify the “singing skill” of thousands of avian species according to the parameters: Loudness or carrying power, Scope (variety and complexity), Continuity (shortness of pauses in a standard performance), Tone quality (shown by narrow bands in a spectrogram), Organization or Order (Gestalt closure, musical coherence), and Imitative ability. Though Hartshorne's analyses were statistically rudimentary, his explicit goal was to relate “song-development” to biologically relevant factors such as behavioral context, plumage coloration, diet, and habitat (Hartshorne, 1973).

Recently, several more investigations of bird song musicality have been conducted with new empirical rigor (Fitch, 2015). Doolittle et al. (2014) and Araya-Salas (2012) compared the frequency ratios used in Hermit Thrush (Catharus guttatus) and Nightingale Wren (Microcerculus philomela) song to common harmonic intervals used in Western tonal music, demonstrating convergence in the first case and its absence in the second. Using human subjects, Doolittle and Brumm (2012) found that synthesized versions of Musician Wren (Cyphorhinus arada) songs that preserved the original intervallic relationships between notes evaluated as more “musical” than songs with slightly deformed tonal relationships. Patel et al. (2009; see also Keehn et al., 2019) reported behavioral evidence of musical beat perception and synchronization in a Sulfur-crested Cockatoo (Cacatua galerita eleonora), the first description of such behavior in a non-human animal. In another rhythmicity study, Roeske et al. (2020) showed that Thrush Nightingale (Luscinia luscinia) rhythms, similar to many human ones, are categorical and centered around small ratios. Earp and Maney (2012) found when female White-throated Sparrows (Zonotrichia albicollis) in a reproductive state listened to song from male conspecifics, immediate early gene activity increased in every region of the mesolimbic reward pathway that shows differential response to music in its putative human homolog.

Many musical signals also exhibit temporal structure at more complex levels of organization. Rothenberg et al. (2014) quantified aspects of “higher order” musicality by using phase and Wiener entropy plots to trace the rhythmic and tonal trajectories of Thrush Nightingale songs, uncovering “escalations” and “modifications” of rhythm and frequency formally similar to some human music. Studying the same species, Roeske et al. (2018) used “multi-fractal analysis” to uncover musical variations in the timing, duration, and intensity of notes across different levels of temporal hierarchy. Along similar lines, Janney et al. (2016) found that Pied Butcherbirds (Cracticus nigrogularis) with more “phrase types” in their repertoires tended to repeat common motifs shared across phrase types more often than those with repertoires composed of fewer phrase types. This implied a repertoire size-dependent optimization of “balance between repetition and novelty” in long song bouts (Janney et al., 2016).

Musical structure at these higher levels may exist in bird song, but the fact cannot be assumed a priori. Musicality is a percept—not a physically definable property of a sound—and humans are the only species from which we can collect direct reports on subjective aesthetic evaluations. So, the best way to test for the presence of musicality in bird song is to use humans as musical feature detectors. As one of the classic ethnomusicology texts states, “All humans can identify music—though not necessarily understand it—when they hear it” (Nettl, 2005:25).

Here, we used human subjects to evaluate whether there is higher-order, musical temporal structure in complex bird songs. By temporal musical structure, we mean time-based variation in acoustic content that elicits a positive hedonic response in the listener. To test for the existence of musical structure, we used the method “music scrambling” from the field of experimental music cognition (Levitin and Menon, 2003, 2005; Abrams et al., 2011), which involves reordering segments of recorded sound so that its temporal structure can be disturbed without significantly altering its global length or total spectral content. Functional MRI studies have shown differential responses to normal and scrambled music in music-sensitive populations of neurons in humans (e.g., Norman-Haignere et al., 2015).

To our knowledge, this is the first general test of the existence of high-order musical temporal structure across a range of avian taxa.

Materials and Methods

Sampling Method

We are not investigating whether all bird songs have high-order musical temporal structure, but whether any bird songs do. Thus, we used a specifically biased sample of highly complex bird songs exhibiting a variety of what we perceived to be musical features. We did not include in our sample any songs that obviously lack complex temporal structure among syllables, such as songs that include a series of identical notes uttered at a continuous pace (e.g., Chipping Sparrow, Spizella passerina). Rather, we selected songs characterized by complex acoustic structure composed of discrete and variable notes or syllables. We also selected bird songs with notes and syllables that were temporally discrete rather than graded and continuous in order to facilitate temporal scrambling without creating obvious acoustic artifacts.

Our sample included single songs or single song bouts from 20 bird species from 13 different families (Table 1). Nineteen samples were songs of male oscine songbirds (Passeri, Passeriformes), and one was a mechanically produced winnowing tail sound from a displaying male Wilson's Snipe (Gallinago delicata; Scolopacidae, Charadriiformes). Recordings were collected from various archival sources (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Experimental stimulus identities and sources.

Stimulus Preparation

High-quality digital audio files of bird songs with minimal background noise were edited using the program Audition CC (Adobe Audition CC, Adobe Systems, San Diego, CA, USA). First, each note or syllable was split into a separate audio file. Each file began at the exact onset of the sound and ended just before the onset of the next note or syllable (i.e., gaps between syllables were grouped with the previous syllable). Terminal syllables were cut off after the end of visually detectable sound in the spectrogram. Editing was done visually following ten Cate and Okanoya (2012). Thus, edited audio files varied in length with the length of the note or syllable from ~6 ms to longer than 1 s. Some recordings from species with comparatively short or fast-paced songs included multiple songs; in these cases, notes/syllables were randomized within individual songs and the periods of silence between songs were preserved. 2 s of relative silence from the original source recording was added before the beginning and after the ending of each song in order to normalize the presentation of stimuli. Envelopes of ~0.2 s were applied to each note/syllable file to reduce boundary artifacts upon recombination. Audition's spectral editing tool was used to decrease background noise, normalize the recordings and remove unwanted sonic artifacts (other bird vocalizations, environmental noise, etc.). The Noise Reduction tool was used to decrease the general background noise, and prominent artifacts were manually removed from the spectrogram.

After each bird song was edited into its component notes/syllables, the edited audio files were recombined into two versions: one in original temporal order, and another with random temporal order (Figures 1, 2). The new recordings were then reviewed once more and converted to mp3 files for uploading onto the online survey platform. All stimulus audio files have been uploaded to Mendeley Data (doi: 10.17632/pkrvf77by8.2). Spectrograms of all stimuli are available in the Supplementary Material.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A, upper panel) Canyon wren (Catherpes mexicanus) recording used in survey, with song elements in original temporal order. (B, lower panel) Same Canyon wren recording, with song elements in randomized temporal order. Spectrograms were created with were created using a 1024-point FFT and a Hamming window with 87.5% overlap.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A, upper panel) Control stimulus from survey. Human fiddle music recording, with notes in original temporal order. (B, lower panel) Same fiddle music recording, with notes in randomized temporal order. Spectrograms were created with were created using a 1024-point FFT and a Hamming window with 87.5% overlap.

Survey Design and Implementation

A psychophysical survey for human subjects was created using the online platform Qualtrics. In the survey, subjects were given the following prompt and instructions:

“Music is often characterized by tunefulness, harmony, temporal patterning, and thematic development. In each question, you will hear a series of two audio recordings. Please indicate which sounds more musical with regard to the qualities listed above.”

The prompt was designed to suggest to the subjects a broad range of criteria that might influence the evaluation of musicality without specifying temporal pattern exclusively. The subjects were then presented with original sequence and temporally randomized recordings from the 20 bird species under review. Two recordings of human music (excerpts of solo fiddle and banjo performances) were manipulated in a similar manner and presented along with the bird song recordings as a control for subject attentiveness or perverse responses. The order of species/human music presentation was randomized, as was the order of natural vs. manipulated stimuli within species. Subjects were required to start the playback of each recording themselves, and a timer was implemented so that it would not be possible to advance to the next sample until the subject had had time to listen to the entire recording.

All subjects provided personal demographic information including: gender; whether they were hearing impaired; whether they had experience identifying wild birds by song; whether they had ever owned pet birds; and how much prior musical experience they had. All subjects were 18 years or older. Following Doolittle and Brumm (2012), the musical experience categories available to participants were:

a.) “little or no experience studying music, singing, or playing an instrument,”

b.) “amateur/some experience studying music, singing, or playing an instrument,” and

c.) “professional/extensive experience studying music, singing, or playing an instrument.”

Summaries of subject demographic and personal experience data are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of survey subjects, N = 92.

Subjects were recruited using Amazon's crowdsourcing marketplace Mechanical Turk, and paid a small fee for their participation. We only accepted subjects who had had at least 90% of their previous MTurk tasks approved, and IP address and geolocation information were used to ensure that duplicate surveys were not counted. On the MTurk website, the activity was entitled “Sound Musicality Survey.” No information provided to the participants mentioned that the sounds were bird songs. The survey took an average of 19 min and 56 s to complete.

Statistical Analysis

We recruited a sample of 126 human subjects. There were 33 subjects who identified either the temporally randomized fiddle or banjo music as more musical than the original temporal sequence recordings. We concluded that they were inattentive or malicious, and they were eliminated from any further consideration (Fleischer et al., 2015). Given the frequency of inattentive responses to our two control questions, we could expect there may be as many as 23 additional inattentive subjects making random choices included in our analysis. One additional observation was excluded because it was a duplicate survey from the same coordinates (latitude and longitude); we kept the first survey taken from that respondent and excluded the second. The processed survey response dataset used in the statistical analysis can be found at the Mendeley Data link provided in Data Availability Statement.

We first assessed whether there was a difference in musicality ratings between the 20 species studied. Each participant evaluated all 20 species, leading to correlation between responses for different species assessed by the same participant. To account for the correlation in musicality evaluations between different species assessed by the same participant, we analyzed the responses from the remaining 92 subjects using a multilevel mixed-effects logistic regression model with the endpoint of correct assignment of original order vs. the endpoint of temporally randomized bird song. Species was included as a fixed effect in the model, participant as a random effect, with musical experience and bird ownership as covariates. To assess whether original order bird songs were more likely to be assessed as more musical by participants overall, we performed a meta-analysis using the proportion of participants choosing original order bird song across all species. All analyses were conducted using Stata 15 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX).

Results

The random effect on our logistic regression model was highly significant (p < 0.0001), suggesting that subjects varied in their musicality ratings of original order vs. temporally randomized songs of this sample of avian species. We also found a highly significant difference in musicality ratings across all bird species studied (p < 0.0001). For 10 of 20 species in the study, the 95% confidence intervals for the average musicality responses both excluded, and were more musical than, the null expectation (Figure 3). For seven of 20 species, the average musicality responses were more musical than, but did not exclude, the null. Only three of 20 species (Field Sparrow, Common Firecrest, and Wilson's Snipe) had average musicality evaluation responses that were less musical than the null expectation, but none of these were statistically distinguishable from the null. Combining the results from each species meta-analytically, bird songs with original temporal sequence were significantly more likely to be evaluated as more musical by human subjects than bird songs with randomized temporal sequence (p < 0.0001).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Proportion of human subjects who rated the original order song of each species as more musical, with 95% confidence intervals ordered from high to low.

Musicality ratings were not affected by either musical experience of the subjects (some experience vs. no experience odds ratio 0.93; 95% C.I. 0.69, 1.26; extensive experience vs. no experience odds ratio 0.68; 95% C.I. 0.33, 1.40; p = 0.6) or by history of pet bird ownership (odds ratio 1.36; 95% C.I. 0.98, 1.88; p = 0.064).

Discussion

We hypothesized that some acoustically complex bird songs have higher order, musical temporal structure. In other words, we hypothesize that syllable sequence is not temporally arbitrary for some birds, but has specifically evolved because of its aesthetic, musical impact upon the receiver. Given that the critical agents in this evolutionary process are birds themselves, it is reasonable to ask why our question was not addressed via behavioral tests on avian subjects. However, our question was whether bird song evinces high-order musical temporal structure at all. This is a subjective question that cannot be answered via behavioral observation. It requires “introspective access” (sensu Bowling and Fitch, 2015) to aesthetic evaluations, which means that only humans can confirm the broad existence of musical structure per se. So, we have used human subjects to evaluate the relative musicality of the specific, original sequence of notes/syllables found in natural songs compared to a randomized sequence of the same notes/syllables. We found strong support for the hypothesis that the temporal sequence of notes/syllables is musically non-random in some bird songs.

Western classical compositions are often characterized as having temporal development, such as introduction, variations on a theme, and creation/resolution of expectations (e.g., Huron, 2006; Morgan et al., 2019). Human psychological research demonstrates that perception of musicality is strongly associated with the presence of natural temporal structure (e.g., Norman-Haignere et al., 2015). This finding supports our conclusion that human subjects can perceive aesthetic temporal structure in some bird songs.

Bird songs are so much shorter in duration than most human musical performances, they may be better compared to musical motives (Hartshorne, 1973). Motives are shorter musical themes or “ideas” that are characterized by a series of notes of particular pitches and rhythmic sequence (Zbikowski, 1999). Motives also have aesthetic structure—i.e., their musical effect would be transformed or disrupted if the sequence of pitches or the amount of time between discrete sonic events were scrambled.

Some examples of musical temporal structure in bird song appear obvious to many listeners. Pied Butcherbirds sing in antiphonal choruses whose interlocking parts resemble the “hocketed” lines of medieval motets and freely improvised jazz (Taylor, 2010). Male Club-winged Manakins (Machaeropterus deliciosus) stridulate their secondary wing feathers during courtship to create sustained, string instrument-like tones (Bostwick and Prum, 2005). Among the species used in our sample, the songs of male Japanese Bush Warbler (Horornis diphone) are famous for their musicality (Hartshorne, 1973). They are characterized by an initial penetrating pure tone or series of tones that increase in volume, and an abrupt cascade of diverse and rapidly modulated pure tones on different frequencies. This temporal sequence of events has a clear analog in the “introduction, variation, and resolution” phrase structure found across many human musical traditions. Perhaps unsurprisingly, human subjects rated the natural song of Japanese Bush Warbler more musical at a higher frequency than nearly every other species in our sample.

When we scramble music, bird song, or speech, what exactly is disrupted for the receiver? In human speech, temporal patterning affects categorical perception. In certain cases, identical sounds are classified as different phonemes depending on their location in the speech stream (Bloch, 1941; Lachlan and Nowicki, 2015). Auditory perception in some songbirds exhibits a similar dependency. Lachlan and Nowicki (2015) studied three related note types in Swamp Sparrow (Melospiza georgiana) song, which they named “short,” “intermediate,” and “long.” The types are easy to separate via acoustic clustering methods, but the study population of Swamp Sparrows perceived only two categories in playback experiments: short and long. In further experiments, the authors determined that “intermediate” notes tended to be classified as “short” if they fell at the beginning of a syllable and “long” if they fell at the end. Roeske et al.'s (2020) discovery of small ratio, categorical rhythms in Thrush Nightingale songs suggests a similar perceptual grounding—scrambling note order redistributes the onset-to-onset interval times in a given song, possibly shifting them toward ratios less typical (and therefore possibly less preferred) for the species.

Above the level of segment sequence in speech is “prosody,” or variation in the frequency, timing, and intensity of elements over the course of a spoken phrase (Mol et al., 2017). Analogous variation in musical phrasing is sometimes called “musical prosody” and can be critical in establishing and manipulating musical expectation in listeners (Huron, 2006; Palmer and Hutchins, 2006; Heffner and Slevc, 2015). Certain songbirds appear sensitive to prosodic cues in human speech as well. In a set of behavioral experiments, Spierings and ten Cate (2014) found that Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata) responded more to prosodic cues in manipulated human speech recordings than syntactic structure—and they responded even more strongly to prosody than human subjects did. Prosodic structure could be a productive study object for vocal evolution research since it is hierarchically organized, common to language and music, and independent from semantic meaning (Mol et al., 2017).

The fact that both birds and humans exhibit temporal pattern-dependent categorical perception and prosody salience is likely due to convergent evolution. But avian possession of these important perceptual building blocks of human musicality suggests that aesthetic perception of sound is likely not restricted to humans.

Origins of Avian Musicality

Why should temporal musicality evolve in bird song? Unlike alarm calls, for example, bird song functions in sexual signaling, and is hypothesized to evolve primarily under sexual selection. We will consider three hypotheses concerning avian acoustic signal evolution: honest signaling theory, sensory/cognitive bias, and arbitrary aesthetic coevolution.

Honest signaling theory suggests that reliable information about signaler quality or condition is insured by the production and survival costs of the signal (Gil and Gahr, 2002). In honest signaling, the adaptive advantage of the song is an extrinsic property that is correlated with, but not part of, the sexual signal such as good genes that will enhance the survival of offspring, material resources necessary for reproduction, minimizing search costs, or protection from sexually transmitted diseases or other infections. In bird song, the potential for encoding information about mate quality in song has been hypothesized to be related to motor constraints, such as the trade-off between trill rate and frequency bandwidth in songbird song elements (e.g., Podos, 1997; Ballentine et al., 2004). However, in general, musical structures do not appear to be strongly shaped by such constraints. Human vocal songs tend to be “dominated by small melodic intervals and simple rhythmic ratios” (Mehr et al., 2019). A small melodic ratio implies a narrow frequency bandwidth between adjacent notes. If human songs were optimized for “performance,” we would expect them to compensate for these small ratios by favoring fast trills by design, at least by the trill rate/bandwidth interpretation of vocal performance. This is plainly untrue, as human song incorporates a variety of speeds and rhythms. Since human song appears to strongly deviate from vocal motor limits, it is “low performance” by design. Therefore, the presence of musical structure in bird song is not predicted by the most prominent avian acoustic application of honest signaling theory.

A second hypothesis is that sensory/cognitive biases emerge in the context of mate choice which arise from independent adaptations or basic design constraints of the sensory and cognitive systems (Ryan and Cummings, 2013; Renoult and Mendelson, 2019). A classic, acoustic example comes from the túngara frog (Physalaemus pustulosus), where the dominant frequencies of male advertisement call components match the previously-evolved tuning of the female inner ear organs (Ryan, 1985; Ryan and Rand, 1990; Ryan et al., 2019). Other biases may be more cognitively rooted, such as the preferences for more complex songs and/or larger repertoires in many oscines, possibly due to an adaptive avoidance of neurological habituation (e.g., Catchpole, 1986; Eda-Fujiwara et al., 2006; Ryan and Cummings, 2013). Importantly, though, evolution of traits due to sensory/cognitive biases alone will not lead to coevolution of traits/preferences. This is because such biases, by definition, are the result of natural selection on unrelated traits.

Alternatively, temporal musical structure in bird song could evolve because it is more aesthetically attractive to learners and receivers than other possible sonic sequences. Aesthetic coevolution involves sensory perception, cognitive evaluation, and choice based on genetically or culturally transmitted variation (Prum, 2012, 2017). More specifically, musical temporal structure in bird songs could evolve as an arbitrary sexually selected trait (Fisher, 1958; Lande, 1981; Kirkpatrick, 1982; Prum, 2010, 2012, 2017; Bailey and Moore, 2012)— i.e., there is no causal correlation between temporal musicality in bird song and signaler quality or condition. Such songs are neither honest nor dishonest because they are unrelated to any extrinsic quality information that can be lied about. Rather, they are merely available for aesthetic evaluation by receivers, and subject to subsequent sexual or social selection. However, the sharing of components of musicality by some complex bird songs and human music implies that these avian acoustic signals are extremely non-random in another way—their aesthetic impact upon the receiver (Rothenberg et al., 2014; Roeske et al., 2020).

To distinguish between arbitrary aesthetic coevolution (Bailey and Moore, 2012; Prum, 2012, 2017) and perceptual bias (Ryan and Cummings, 2013; Renoult and Mendelson, 2019), we need evidence of coevolution of preferences and traits. Evidence suggesting such a dynamic is abundant for oscine birds. Classic studies of male neural/cognitive templates, or learning biases, demonstrate that heritable, biologically evolving neural preferences can coevolve with the vocal structure of male song (e.g., Nottebohm, 1968, 1970; Marler and Waser, 1977; Marler and Sherman, 1983; Lachlan and Feldman, 2003). On the other extreme, Derryberry (2007) conducted a playback experiment on the White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys)—a species with a song that was ranked as having with among the most musical temporal structure in our sample (Figure 3). In a population in the Sierra Nevada, California, Derryberry played song recordings to wild female and male sparrows of contemporaneous male songs, and male songs recorded 24 years earlier at the same locality. She found that the older songs elicited nearly half the social response—either male territorial challenges, or curious female interest—as the contemporaneous songs did. In other words, cultural evolution in male White-crowned Sparrow song was associated with corresponding cultural coevolution in the social salience and attractiveness of those social signals. These data cannot be explained by a sensory bias alone.

However, the ability of both humans (in our experiments) and birds (avian evaluators in wild populations of these species) to perceive and prefer musical temporal structures in bird songs does imply the independent evolution of some broad cognitive preference for temporal aesthetic structure has evolved convergently in multiple different lineages of organisms, minimally including oscine birds and humans. This aesthetic concept could be defined as a kind of broad, non-adaptive, aesthetic cognitive bias—specifically a bias toward being aesthetically engaged by the attraction of attention, and the building and fulfillment of expectation. In this way, we can conceptualize the evolution of temporal musicality in bird song as the result of an interaction between arbitrary sexual selection and broad cognitive biases for aesthetically attractive temporal structure in acoustic sexual signals. Multiple lineages of oscine birds have independently evolved songs with higher-order musical temporal structure as they reached a certain threshold of acoustic complexity and strength of selection (Devoogd et al., 1993). Future work on aesthetic evolution in bird song should work to characterize the nature of acoustic aesthetic biases by comparing multiple evolutionary origins of complex, higher-order temporal structure to close relatives that lack such complexity.

Our explicitly aesthetic hypothesis for the evolution of bird song's musicality continues the tradition of Darwin (1871), who characterized male oscine song as “having the power to charm the female.” A century later, Hartshorne (1973) proposed the “monotony threshold hypothesis,” which proposes that vocal repertoire diversity evolves to prevent habituation—i.e., boredom—in the receiver. The monotony threshold suggests an inverse relationship between “continuity” and “variety” in bird song. Hartshorne posited that birds with more elaborate repertoires tend to sing more continuously than birds with simpler, more repetitive vocalizations. Vocal learning plays a role here as well: species that learn their songs tend to have larger repertoires than those who do not (Marler, 2000).

Variation in Musical Temporal Structure Among Species

Empirical data already document that there are significant variations among species in bird song's musical acoustic structure. Some species appear to explore harmonic content, whereas others explore rhythmic variation (e.g., Doolittle et al., 2014; Rothenberg et al., 2014; Roeske et al., 2020). Although our study was not designed to investigate differential musical temporal structure among species, our data do yield some useful comparative observations. The four species with the highest perceived musical temporal structure in our study were Japanese Bush Warbler (Horornis diphone), Gray Shrikethrush (Colluricincla harmonica), Lazuli Bunting (Passerina amoena), and White-crowned Sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). The songs of the latter three were complex along Hartshorne's (1973) dimensions of “singing skill,” described above for the Japanese Bush Warbler—they employed a variety of pure tone and broadband frequency elements, used a high degree of rhythmic variation, and had, to our ears, a sense of musical “development” and “resolution” over the course of an individual song.

The temporal complexity of these “highly musical” songs stood in noticeable contrast to the temporal structure of the songs of the three “least musical” species in our study—Field Sparrow (Spizella pusilla), Common Firecrest (Regulus ignicapilla), and Wilson's Snipe (Gallinago delicata). All three species have songs with a clear sense of rhythmic acceleration (or accelerando in Western musical terms), but they had much less spectral diversity. They were selected for the sample because they might manifest some minimal criteria for musical temporal structure. The Field Sparrow song begins with a rhythmic diminution, but its terminal trill simply repeats the same downward-sweeping note. A similar musical simplicity characterized the song of the Common Firecrest, which consists of a single repeated rhythm accelerating toward the end, with a rise in pitch as its only harmonic development. Wilson's Snipe received the lowest marks for musicality of any species, though this may be due to a unique interaction between the idiosyncrasies of its winnowing tail feather song and the nature of our acoustic manipulation. The song employs a steadily accelerating pulse of notes and features a bell curve-like pitch and loudness contour. When the notes were temporally randomized, however, this smooth progression of pitches and volumes was transformed into something that sounded far more syncopated (almost funky) to us, and apparently more musical to many human observers. In this case, it seems possible that our random manipulation created interesting musical temporal structure instead of breaking it down. Of course, the failure of these songs to be ranked as highly musical in our scrambling experiment does not mean that they are not actually examples of musical temporal structure. An alternative experiment could compare these natural songs to edited recordings of the same notes in the same temporal sequence but at a single, consistent tempo. A negative control (i.e., two alternative random orders of the same non-musical notes) could also be useful for future studies.

These examples also do not imply that bird song always sounds more musical as it gets more complex. The song of the Winter Wren (Troglodytes troglodytes) is an intricate stream of varied sounds and rhythms, but it received middling musicality scores in our study. The vagaries of individual aesthetic preference and differences in acoustic perception between humans and birds (such as the fact that birds can discern changes in the temporal structure of harmonic sounds at a much finer timescale than humans) make it impossible to render objective comparative judgements about avian musicality across species based on acoustic features alone (Hartshorne, 1973; Lohr et al., 2006). The likely presence of systemic biases in the musical preferences of our study subjects—who were by definition tech-savvy computer owners—underscores this point. In future studies, introducing a control featuring human vocal recordings (which are maximally analogous to bird song in terms of biomechanical production) could further validate the experimental method.

We think that human evaluations of bird musicality are scientifically informative. Indeed, if it were not for the aesthetic evaluation by humans of other species' display traits, the field of sexual selection would not exist at all. Darwin became “sick” at the sight of an eyespot on a Peacock's tail feather because he found it uselessly beautiful (Burkhardt et al., 1995). If the tail had not appeared beautiful to him, there would have been no need to seek a novel evolutionary mechanism to explain its visual aesthetics. Musical beauty is no different. Although there are clearly limits to the observational potential of human evaluators, using humans to establish the existence of high-order musical temporal structure in bird song is within these bounds, and effective.

Data Availability Statement

All stimuli audio files and processed survey results are available for download at Mendeley Data (doi: 10.17632/pkrvf77by8.2 ).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed by the Yale University Human Subjects Committee, and were granted an exemption from permit and informed consent requirements.

Author Contributions

RP and HB conceived of the study and wrote the manuscript. HB created the stimuli, designed the psychophysical survey, and collected the data. EV and AV carried out the statistical analyses. KK designed and wrote Bird Call, the Max MSP patch used to recombine bird song notes/syllables. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the W. R. Coe Fund, Yale University and the Franke Program in Science and the Humanities, Yale University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

J. Clarke and M. Ryan provided useful discussion of the manuscript. The comments of T. Freeberg, S. Handel, and two anonymous reviewers improved an earlier version of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.629456/full#supplementary-material

References

Abrams, D. A., Bhatara, A., Ryali, S., Balaban, E., Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2011). Decoding temporal structure in music and speech relies on shared brain resources but elicits different fine-scale spatial patterns. Cereb. Cort. 21, 1507–1518. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamic, L. (2011). Unzipping Zipf's law. Nature 474, 164–165. doi: 10.1038/474164a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araya-Salas, M. (2012). Is birdsong music? Evaluating harmonic intervals in songs of a Neotropical songbird. An. Beh. 84, 309–313. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, E. A. (1963). A Study of Bird Song. London: Oxford University Press.

Bailey, N. W., and Moore, A. J. (2012). Runaway sexual selection without genetic correlations: social environments and flexible mate choice initiate and enhance the Fisher process. Evolution 66, 2674–2684. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01647.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballentine, B., Hyman, J., and Nowicki, S. (2004). Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav. Ecol. 15, 163–168. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berwick, R. C., Okanoya, K., Beckers, G. J., and Bolhuis, J. J. (2011). Songs to syntax: the linguistics of birdsong. Tr. Cog. Sci. 15, 113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloch, B. (1941). Phonemic overlapping. Amer. Sp. 16, 278–284. doi: 10.2307/486567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Birdsong, Speech, and Language: Exploring the Evolution of Mind and Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9322.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text

Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K., and Scharff, C. (2010). Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nat. Rev. Neur. 11, 747–759. doi: 10.1038/nrn2931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bostwick, K. S., and Prum, R. O. (2005). Courting bird sings with stridulating wing feathers. Science 309, 736–736. doi: 10.1126/science.1111701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowling, D. L., and Fitch, W. T. (2015). Do animal communication systems have phonemes? Tr. Cog. Sci. 19, 555–557. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkhardt, F., Porter, D., Browne, J., and Richmond, M. (1995). The Correspondence of Charles Darwin, Vol. 8, 1860. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Catchpole, C. K. (1986). Song repertoires and reproductive success in the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus. Behav. Ecol. Soc. 19, 439–445. doi: 10.1007/BF00300547

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, W. (1943). The Song of the Wood Pewee (Myiochanes virens Linnaeus): A Study of Bird Music. Albany: New York State Museum Bulletin No. 334.

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: Murray. doi: 10.5962/bhl.title.2092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derryberry, E. P. (2007). Evolution of bird song affects signal efficacy: an experimental test using historical and current signals. Evolution 61, 1938–1945. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devoogd, T. J., Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D., and Purvis, A. (1993). Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscine birds. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lon. Bio. Sci. 254, 75–82. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1993.0129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doolittle, E., and Brumm, H. (2012). O Canto do Uirapuru: consonant intervals and patterns in the song of the musician wren. Jour. Int. Mus. Stud. 6, 55–85. doi: 10.4407/jims.2013.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doolittle, E. L., Gingras, B., Endres, D. M., and Fitch, W. T. (2014). Overtone-based pitch selection in hermit thrush song: unexpected convergence with scale construction in human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 16616–16621. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1406023111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doupe, A. J., and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Ann. Rev. Neu. 22, 567–631. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earp, S. E., and Maney, D. L. (2012). Birdsong: is it music to their ears?. Front. Evol. Neur. 4:14. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eda-Fujiwara, H., Satoh, R., and Miyamoto, T. (2006). Song preferences by females: male song complexity and gene expression in the female brain. Orn. Sci. 5, 23–29. doi: 10.2326/osj.5.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. A. (1958). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Dover.

Google Scholar

Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Bio. Sci. 370:20140091. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleischer, A., Mead, A. D., and Huang, J. (2015). Inattentive responding in MTurk and other online samples. Ind. Org. Psyc. 8:196. doi: 10.1017/iop.2015.25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, D., and Gahr, M. (2002). The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Tr. Ecol. Evol. 17, 133–141. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02410-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartshorne, C. (1958). The relation of bird song to music. Ibi 100, 421–445. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1958.tb00406.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartshorne, C. (1973). Born to Sing: An Interpretation and World Survey of Bird Song. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Google Scholar

Heffner, C. C., and Slevc, L. R. (2015). Prosodic structure as a parallel to musical structure. Front. Psy. 6:1962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huron, D. B. (2006). Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/6575.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyland Bruno, J., Jarvis, E. D., Liberman, M., and Tchernichovski, O. (2020). Birdsong learning and culture: analogies with human spoken language. Ann. Rev. Ling. 7, 449–472. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-090420-121034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janney, E., Taylor, H., Scharff, C., Rothenberg, D., Parra, L. C., and Tchernichovski, O. (2016). Temporal regularity increases with repertoire complexity in the Australian pied butcherbird's song. Roy. Soc. Op. Sci. 3:160357. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarvis, E. D. (2019). Evolution of vocal learning and spoken language. Science 366, 50–54. doi: 10.1126/science.aax0287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keehn, R. J. J., Iversen, J. R., Schulz, I., and Patel, A. D. (2019). Spontaneity and diversity of movement to music are not uniquely human. Curr. Biol. 29, R621–R622. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keister, R. A., and Baptista, L. F. (2005). Why birdsong is sometimes like music. Pers. Bio. Med. 48, 426–443. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2005.0066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkpatrick, M. (1982). Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution 36, 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachlan, R. F., and Feldman, M. W. (2003). Evolution of cultural communication systems: the coevolution of cultural signals and genes encoding learning preferences. J. Evol. Biol. 16, 1084–1095. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00624.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachlan, R. F., and Nowicki, S. (2015). Context-dependent categorical perception in a songbird. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 1892–1897. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1410844112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lande, R. (1981). Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 78, 3721–3725. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.6.3721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2003). Musical structure is processed in “language” areas of the brain: a possible role for Brodmann Area 47 in temporal coherence. NeuroImage 20, 2142–2152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2005). The neural locus of temporal structure and expectancies in music: evidence from functional neuroimaging at 3 Tesla. Mus. Perc. 22, 563–575. doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.563

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipkind, D., Marcus, G. F., Bemis, D. K., Sasahara, K., Jacoby, N., Takahasi, M., et al. (2013). Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 498, 104–108. doi: 10.1038/nature12173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohr, B., Dooling, R. J., and Bartone, S. (2006). The discrimination of temporal fine structure in call-like harmonic sounds by birds. J. Comp. Psy. 120:239. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marler, P. (2000). “Origins of music and speech: insights from animals,” in The Origins of Music, eds N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 31–48.

Google Scholar

Marler, P., Evans, C. S., and Hauser, M. D. (1992). “Animal signals: motivational, referential, or both?,” in Nonverbal Vocal Communication: Comparative and Developmental Approaches, eds H. Papoušek, U. Jürgens, and M. Papoušek (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 66–86.

Google Scholar

Marler, P., and Sherman, V. (1983). Song structure without auditory feedback: emendations of the auditory template hypothesis. J. Neu. 3, 517–531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-03-00517.1983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marler, P., and Waser, M. S. (1977). Role of auditory feedback in canary song development. J. Comp. Phys. Psy. 91, 8–16. doi: 10.1037/h0077303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., Knox, D., Ketter, D. M., Pickens-Jones, D., Atwood, S., et al. (2019). Universality and diversity in human song. Science 366:6468. doi: 10.1126/science.aax0868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mol, C., Chen, A., Kager, R. W., and ter Haar, S. M. (2017). Prosody in birdsong: a review and perspective. Neu. Bio. Rev. 81, 167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, E., Fogel, A., Nair, A., and Patel, A. D. (2019). Statistical learning and Gestalt-like principles predict melodic expectations. Cognition 189, 23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nettl, B. (2005). The Study of Ethnomusicology: Thirty-One Issues and Concepts. Champaign: University of Illinois Press.

Google Scholar

Norman-Haignere, S., Kanwisher, N. G., and McDermott, J. H. (2015). Distinct cortical pathways for music and speech revealed by hypothesis-free voxel decomposition. Neuron 88, 1281–1296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nottebohm, F. (1968). Auditory experience and song development in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Ibi 110, 549–568. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1968.tb00063.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nottebohm, F. (1970). Ontogeny of bird song. Science 167, 950–956. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3920.950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, C., and Hutchins, S. (2006). What is musical prosody?. Psy. Lea. Mot. 46, 245–278. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(06)46007-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Bregman, M. R., and Schulz, I. (2009). Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr. Biol. 19, 827–830. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51, 537–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prum, R. O. (2010). The Lande–Kirkpatrick mechanism is the null model of evolution by intersexual selection: implications for meaning, honesty, and design in intersexual signals. Evolution 64, 3085–3100. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01054.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prum, R. O. (2012). Aesthetic evolution by mate choice: Darwin's really dangerous idea. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. Bio. Sci. 367, 2253–2265. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prum, R. O. (2017). The Evolution of Beauty: How Darwin's Forgotten Theory of Mate Choice Shapes the Animal World—and Us. New York, NY: Doubleday.

Google Scholar

Renoult, J. P., and Mendelson, T. C. (2019). Processing bias: extending sensory drive to include efficacy and efficiency in information processing. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. Bio. Sci. 286:20190165. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeske, T. C., Kelty-Stephen, D., and Wallot, S. (2018). Multifractal analysis reveals music-like dynamic structure in songbird rhythms. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-22933-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeske, T. C., Tchernichovski, O., Poeppel, D., and Jacoby, N. (2020). Categorical rhythms are shared between songbirds and humans. Curr. Biol. 30, 3544–3555. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothenberg, D., Roeske, T. C., Voss, H. U., Naguib, M., and Tchernichovski, O. (2014). Investigation of musicality in birdsong. Hear. Res. 308, 71–83. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. J. (1985). The Túngara Frog: A Study in Sexual Selection and Communication. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. J., Akre, K. L., Baugh, A. T., Bernal, X. E., Lea, A. M., Leslie, C., et al. (2019). Nineteen years of consistently positive and strong female mate preferences despite individual variation. Am. Nat. 194, 125–134. doi: 10.1086/704103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. J., and Cummings, M. E. (2013). Perceptual biases and mate choice. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Sys. 44, 437–459. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, M. J., and Rand, A. S. (1990). The sensory basis of sexual selection for complex calls in the túngara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus (sexual selection for sensory exploitation). Evolution 44, 305–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savage, P. E., Brown, S., Sakai, E., and Currie, T. E. (2015). Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 8987–8992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414495112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spierings, M. J., and ten Cate, C. (2014). Zebra finches are sensitive to prosodic features of human speech. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. Bio. Sci. 281:20140480. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stap, D. (2005). Birdsong. New York, NY: Scribner.

Google Scholar

Stumpf, C. (1911). The Origins of Music. (Trans. Trippett D). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Taylor, H. (2010). Blowin'in birdland: improvisation and the Australian pied butcherbird. Leo. Mus. J. 20, 79–83. doi: 10.1162/LMJ_a_00016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ten Cate, C., and Okanoya, K. (2012). Revisiting the syntactic abilities of non-human animals: natural vocalizations and artificial grammar learning. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. Bio. Sci. 367, 1984–1994. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, R. F., and Clarke, J. (1975). “1/f noise” in music and speech. Nature 258, 317–318. doi: 10.1038/258317a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zbikowski, L. M. (1999). Musical coherence, motive, and categorization. Mus. Perc. 17, 5–42. doi: 10.2307/40285810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bio-musicology, musicality, linguistics, bird song, sexual selection, honest signaling, perceptual bias, aesthetic evolution

Citation: Bilger HT, Vertosick E, Vickers A, Kaczmarek K and Prum RO (2021) Higher-Order Musical Temporal Structure in Bird Song. Front. Psychol. 12:629456. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.629456

Received: 14 November 2020; Accepted: 25 February 2021;
Published: 25 March 2021.

Edited by:

Julia Hyland Bruno, Columbia University, United States

Reviewed by:

Dave Gammon, Elon University, United States
Erich David Jarvis, Duke University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Bilger, Vertosick, Vickers, Kaczmarek and Prum. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hans T. Bilger, hansbilger@utexas.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.